Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Parallel encoding of recent visual experience and self-motion during navigation in Drosophila

Abstract

Animal navigation requires multiple types of information for decisions on directional heading. We identified neural processing channels that encode multiple cues during navigational decision-making in Drosophila melanogaster. In a flight simulator, we found that flies made directional choices on the basis of the location of a recently presented landmark. This experience-guided navigation was impaired by silencing neurons in the bulb (BU), a region in the central brain. Two-photon calcium imaging during flight revealed that the dorsal part of the BU encodes the location of a recent landmark, whereas the ventral part of the BU tracks self-motion reflecting turns. Photolabeling-based circuit tracing indicated that these functional compartments of the BU constitute adjacent, yet distinct, anatomical pathways that both enter the navigation center. Thus, the fly's navigation system organizes multiple types of information in parallel channels, which may compactly transmit signals without interference for decision-making during flight.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Information about the location of a recent cue guides flight orientation.
Figure 2: The BU is involved in experience-guided flight orientation.
Figure 3: Two populations of microglomeruli in the BU show distinct task-related activity.
Figure 4: Two populations of microglomeruli selectively encode recent visual experience and self-motion, respectively.
Figure 5: Two populations of microglomeruli are spatially segregated.
Figure 6: Dorsal and ventral parts of the BU constitute parallel pathways.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Collett, T.S. & Collett, M. Memory use in insect visual navigation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 542–552 (2002).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Collett, T.S. & Graham, P. Animal navigation: path integration, visual landmarks and cognitive maps. Curr. Biol. 14, R475–R477 (2004).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Dudchenko, P.A. An overview of the tasks used to test working memory in rodents. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 28, 699–709 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Neuser, K., Triphan, T., Mronz, M., Poeck, B. & Strauss, R. Analysis of a spatial orientation memory in Drosophila. Nature 453, 1244–1247 (2008).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Collett, T.S. & Collett, M. Path integration in insects. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 10, 757–762 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. McNaughton, B.L., Battaglia, F.P., Jensen, O., Moser, E.I. & Moser, M.B. Path integration and the neural basis of the 'cognitive map'. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 663–678 (2006).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Seelig, J.D. & Jayaraman, V. Feature detection and orientation tuning in the Drosophila central complex. Nature 503, 262–266 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Sun, Y. et al. Neural signatures of dynamic stimulus selection in Drosophila. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 1104–1113 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Guo, C. et al. A conditioned visual orientation requires the ellipsoid body in Drosophila. Learn. Mem. 22, 56–63 (2014).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Kuntz, S., Poeck, B., Sokolowski, M.B. & Strauss, R. The visual orientation memory of Drosophila requires Foraging (PKG) upstream of Ignorant (RSK2) in ring neurons of the central complex. Learn. Mem. 19, 337–340 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Ofstad, T.A., Zuker, C.S. & Reiser, M.B. Visual place learning in Drosophila melanogaster. Nature 474, 204–207 (2011).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Pfeiffer, K. & Homberg, U. Organization and functional roles of the central complex in the insect brain. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 59, 165–184 (2014).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Solanki, N., Wolf, R. & Heisenberg, M. Central complex and mushroom bodies mediate novelty choice behavior in Drosophila. J. Neurogenet. 29, 30–37 (2015).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Hanesch, U., Fischbach, K.F. & Heisenberg, M. Neuronal architecture of the central complex in Drosophila melanogaster. Cell Tissue Res. 257, 343–366 (1989).

    Google Scholar 

  15. Omoto, J.J. et al. Visual input to the Drosophila central complex by developmentally and functionally distinct neuronal populations. Curr. Biol. 27, 1098–1110 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Young, J.M. & Armstrong, J.D. Structure of the adult central complex in Drosophila: organization of distinct neuronal subsets. J. Comp. Neurol. 518, 1500–1524 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Green, J. et al. A neural circuit architecture for angular integration in Drosophila. Nature 546, 101–106 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Kim, S.S., Rouault, H., Druckmann, S. & Jayaraman, V. Ring attractor dynamics in the Drosophila central brain. Science 356, 849–853 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Seelig, J.D. & Jayaraman, V. Neural dynamics for landmark orientation and angular path integration. Nature 521, 186–191 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Turner-Evans, D. et al. Angular velocity integration in a fly heading circuit. Elife 6, e23496 (2017).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Maimon, G., Straw, A.D. & Dickinson, M.H. Active flight increases the gain of visual motion processing in Drosophila. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 393–399 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Seelig, J.D. et al. Two-photon calcium imaging from head-fixed Drosophila during optomotor walking behavior. Nat. Methods 7, 535–540 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Gotz, K.G. Course-control, metabolism and wing interference during ultralong tethered flight in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Exp. Biol. 128, 35–46 (1987).

    Google Scholar 

  24. Antunes, M. & Biala, G. The novel object recognition memory: neurobiology, test procedure, and its modifications. Cogn. Process. 13, 93–110 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Dill, M. & Heisenberg, M. Visual pattern memory without shape recognition. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 349, 143–152 (1995).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Kennedy, J.S. Zigzagging and casting as a programmed response to wind-borne odor: a review. Physiol. Entomol. 8, 109–120 (1983).

    Google Scholar 

  27. Buchanan, S.M., Kain, J.S. & de Bivort, B.L. Neuronal control of locomotor handedness in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 112, 6700–6705 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Ito, M., Masuda, N., Shinomiya, K., Endo, K. & Ito, K. Systematic analysis of neural projections reveals clonal composition of the Drosophila brain. Curr. Biol. 23, 644–655 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Wong, D.C. et al. Postembryonic lineages of the Drosophila brain. II. Identification of lineage projection patterns based on MARCM clones. Dev. Biol. 384, 258–289 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Yu, H.H. et al. Clonal development and organization of the adult Drosophila central brain. Curr. Biol. 23, 633–643 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Weir, P.T. & Dickinson, M.H. Functional divisions for visual processing in the central brain of flying Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 112, E5523–E5532 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Gotz, K.G., Hengstenberg, B. & Biesinger, R. Optomotor control of wing beat and body posture in Drosophila. Biol. Cybern. 35, 101–112 (1979).

    Google Scholar 

  33. Tammero, L.F., Frye, M.A. & Dickinson, M.H. Spatial organization of visuomotor reflexes in Drosophila. J. Exp. Biol. 207, 113–122 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Renn, S.C. et al. Genetic analysis of the Drosophila ellipsoid body neuropil: organization and development of the central complex. J. Neurobiol. 41, 189–207 (1999).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Koenig, S., Wolf, R. & Heisenberg, M. Visual attention in flies: dopamine in the mushroom bodies mediates the after-effect of cueing. PLoS One 11, e0161412 (2016).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Koenig, S., Wolf, R. & Heisenberg, M. Vision in flies: measuring the attention span. PLoS One 11, e0148208 (2016).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Sareen, P., Wolf, R. & Heisenberg, M. Attracting the attention of a fly. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 108, 7230–7235 (2011).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Barak, O. & Tsodyks, M. Working models of working memory. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 25, 20–24 (2014).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Wang, X.J. Synaptic reverberation underlying mnemonic persistent activity. Trends Neurosci. 24, 455–463 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Martin, J.P., Guo, P., Mu, L., Harley, C.M. & Ritzmann, R.E. Central-complex control of movement in the freely walking cockroach. Curr. Biol. 25, 2795–2803 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Kuntz, S., Poeck, B. & Strauss, R. Visual working memory requires permissive and instructive NO/cGMP signaling at presynapses in the Drosophila central brain. Curr. Biol. 27, 613–623 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Turner-Evans, D.B. & Jayaraman, V. The insect central complex. Curr. Biol. 26, R453–R457 (2016).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Kim, A.J., Fenk, L.M., Lyu, C. & Maimon, G. Quantitative predictions orchestrate visual signaling in. Drosophila. Cell 168, 280–294 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Kim, A.J., Fitzgerald, J.K. & Maimon, G. Cellular evidence for efference copy in Drosophila visuomotor processing. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1247–1255 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Schnell, B., Ros, I.G. & Dickinson, M.H. A descending neuron correlated with the rapid steering maneuvers of flying Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 27, 1200–1205 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Wolff, T., Iyer, N.A. & Rubin, G.M. Neuroarchitecture and neuroanatomy of the Drosophila central complex: a GAL4-based dissection of protocerebral bridge neurons and circuits. J. Comp. Neurol. 523, 997–1037 (2015).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Kropff, E., Carmichael, J.E., Moser, M.B. & Moser, E.I. Speed cells in the medial entorhinal cortex. Nature 523, 419–424 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Spellman, T. et al. Hippocampal-prefrontal input supports spatial encoding in working memory. Nature 522, 309–314 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Taube, J.S. The head direction signal: origins and sensory-motor integration. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 30, 181–207 (2007).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Vanin, S. et al. Unexpected features of Drosophila circadian behavioral rhythms under natural conditions. Nature 484, 371–375 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Bhandawat, V., Maimon, G., Dickinson, M.H. & Wilson, R.I. Olfactory modulation of flight in Drosophila is sensitive, selective and rapid. J. Exp. Biol. 213, 3625–3635 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Pfeiffer, B.D., Truman, J.W. & Rubin, G.M. Using translational enhancers to increase transgene expression in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 109, 6626–6631 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Paradis, S., Sweeney, S.T. & Davis, G.W. Homeostatic control of presynaptic release is triggered by postsynaptic membrane depolarization. Neuron 30, 737–749 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Pfeiffer, B.D. et al. Tools for neuroanatomy and neurogenetics in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 105, 9715–9720 (2008).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Pfeiffer, B.D. et al. Refinement of tools for targeted gene expression in Drosophila. Genetics 186, 735–755 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Chen, T.W. et al. Ultrasensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature 499, 295–300 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Yang, M.Y., Armstrong, J.D., Vilinsky, I., Strausfeld, N.J. & Kaiser, K. Subdivision of the Drosophila mushroom bodies by enhancer-trap expression patterns. Neuron 15, 45–54 (1995).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Straw, A.D. & Dickinson, M.H. Motmot, an open-source toolkit for real-time video acquisition and analysis. Source Code Biol. Med. 4, 5 (2009).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Reiser, M.B. & Dickinson, M.H. A modular display system for insect behavioral neuroscience. J. Neurosci. Methods 167, 127–139 (2008).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Heisenberg, M. & Wolf, R. Vision in Drosophila: Genetics of Microbehavior (Springer-Verlag, 1984).

  61. Maimon, G., Straw, A.D. & Dickinson, M.H. A simple vision-based algorithm for decision making in flying Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 18, 464–470 (2008).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Tammero, L.F. & Dickinson, M.H. The influence of visual landscape on the free flight behavior of the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. J. Exp. Biol. 205, 327–343 (2002).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Bender, J.A. & Dickinson, M.H. Visual stimulation of saccades in magnetically tethered Drosophila. J. Exp. Biol. 209, 3170–3182 (2006).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Heisenberg, M. & Wolf, R. Reafferent control of optomotor yaw torque in Drosophilamelanogaster. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 163, 373–388 (1988).

    Google Scholar 

  65. Badel, L., Ohta, K., Tsuchimoto, Y. & Kazama, H. Decoding of context-dependent olfactory behavior in Drosophila. Neuron 91, 155–167 (2016).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Schnell, B., Weir, P.T., Roth, E., Fairhall, A.L. & Dickinson, M.H. Cellular mechanisms for integral feedback in visually guided behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 111, 5700–5705 (2014).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Datta, S.R. et al. The Drosophila pheromone cVA activates a sexually dimorphic neural circuit. Nature 452, 473–477 (2008).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Patterson, G.H. & Lippincott-Schwartz, J. A photoactivatable GFP for selective photolabeling of proteins and cells. Science 297, 1873–1877 (2002).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Ruta, V. et al. A dimorphic pheromone circuit in Drosophila from sensory input to descending output. Nature 468, 686–690 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Schindelin, J. et al. Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nat. Methods 9, 676–682 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Feierstein, C.E., Quirk, M.C., Uchida, N., Sosulski, D.L. & Mainen, Z.F. Representation of spatial goals in rat orbitofrontal cortex. Neuron 51, 495–507 (2006).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Britten, K.H., Shadlen, M.N., Newsome, W.T. & Movshon, J.A. The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance. J. Neurosci. 12, 4745–4765 (1992).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Jenett, A. et al. A GAL4-driver line resource for Drosophila neurobiology. Cell Rep. 2, 991–1001 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Y. Aso, G. Rubin, R. Wilson and G. Davis for gifts of fly stocks, K. Ohta for the backcross of flies, L. Badel for help in developing task control programs, G. Maimon for advice on preparation, RIKEN BSI-Olympus Collaboration Center for imaging equipment and software, members of the Kazama laboratory for their support and comments on the manuscript, and S. Fujisawa, Q. Gaudry, A. Nose, N. Uchida and C. Yokoyama for comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by the RIKEN Special Postdoctoral Researchers Program (H.M.S.), JSPS KAKENHI Grant Numbers JP23680044, JP25115732 (H.K.), JP25750410, JP15K18352 (H.M.S.), and RIKEN Brain Science Institute (H.K.).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

H.M.S. collected and analyzed the data. H.M.S. and H.K. designed the study and wrote the manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Hiroshi M Shiozaki or Hokto Kazama.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Integrated supplementary information

Supplementary Figure 1 Characteristics of orientation behavior and the effects of a recent landmark.

(a–c) Bar choice experiment. (a) Task and stimuli. (b) Trajectories of the example fly shown in Fig. 1e during the choice period. One trial (gray line in the right panel) was discarded from further analyses (see Online Methods for the criteria for trial disposal). (c) Population summary (N = 20 flies). Each dot represents the percentage of “left” choice trials for a fly, calculated from 20 trials in each bar location. Choices were biased toward the bar side when one bar was presented (***P < 0.001/2, one-sample t-test with Bonferroni correction, the denominator represents the number of comparisons) but at random when two bars were simultaneously presented (Both, P = 0.60, one-sample t-test). Black lines, mean ± s.e.m. across flies. (d) Time course of trial-averaged ΔWBA during the cueing experiment shown in Fig. 1d–f (N = 20 flies, mean ± s.e.m. across flies). Inset is an enlarged view around the onset of the choice period. (e) Mean ΔWBA during the cue and the delay periods during the cueing experiment (N = 20 flies). Each dot represents the mean ΔWBA for a fly. Flies attempted to turn toward the cue during the cue period (***P < 0.001, one-sample t-test). Black lines, mean ± s.e.m. across flies. (f,g) Cue duration experiment. (f) Task. (g) Summary of choice bias for each cue duration (N = 20 flies). Same format as in e but the ordinate represents the percentage of trials in which the fly chose to orient toward the side where the cue had not been presented. *P < 0.05/5, **P < 0.01/5, ***P < 0.001/5, one-sample t-test with Bonferroni correction. (h,i) Delay duration experiment. (h) Task. (i) Summary of choice bias for each delay (N = 20 flies). Same format as in g. *P < 0.05/4, **P < 0.01/4, ***P < 0.001/4, one-sample t-test with Bonferroni correction. (j–l) Cueing and single bar location experiment. (j) Task. (k) Stimulus set. (l) Summary of choice bias for each condition (N = 20 flies). Same format as in e but the ordinate represents the percentage of trials in which the fly chose to orient toward the bar side. *P < 0.05/3, **P < 0.01/3, ***P < 0.001/3, one-sample t-test with Bonferroni correction. #P < 0.05, ###P < 0.001, one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by post hoc Tukey’s honestly significant difference (HSD) test. (m,n) Closed-loop cue experiment. (m) Task. (n) Summary of choice bias (N = 20 flies). Same format as in g. ***P < 0.001, one-sample t-test. (o,p) Cueing experiment with bright bars. (o) Task. (p) Summary of choice bias (N = 20 flies). Same format as in g. **P < 0.01, one-sample t-test. (q,r) Bar choice experiment with bright bars. (q) Task. (r) Summary of bar choice (N = 20 flies). Same format as in l. The choices were not different from the chance level (P = 0.15, one-sample t-test). (s–u) Bar location experiment. (s) Task. (t) Stimulus set. (u) Summary of choice bias for each bar location in the bar location experiment where only one bar was presented in the closed-loop mode at various locations (N = 20 flies). The degree of choice bias depended on bar location (P < 0.001, one-way ANOVA) with the strongest bias at ±60°. Same format as in l. **P < 0.01/5, ***P < 0.001/5, one-sample t-test with Bonferroni correction. (v) Mean ΔWBA during the cue and delay periods in cue/target location experiment shown in Fig. 1g,h (mean ± s.e.m. across flies; N = 20 flies for each combination of cue and target locations). These values were used to assess the motor hypothesis with partial correlation analysis (see Online Methods). Exact P values for all statistical tests can be found in Supplementary Table 1.

Supplementary Figure 2 Visually-guided flight orientation in flies with inactivated ring neurons.

(a) Expression patterns of the potassium channel in the ventral nerve cord of the three driver lines shown in Fig. 2b. Scale bar, 100 μm. (b) Summary of the choices in the bar choice experiment with two bars (N = 20 flies for each genotype). Each dot represents the percentage of trials in which the fly chose to orient toward the left side. Black lines, mean ± s.e.m. across flies. Variances of % “left” choice among flies did not depend on genotypes (P > 0.05/3 for all pairs of mutant and control genotypes, Bartlett’s test with Bonferroni correction, the denominator represents the number of comparisons). (c) Summary of the choices in the bar choice experiment with a single bar (N = 20 flies for each genotype). The same format as in b but the ordinate represents the percentage of trials in which the fly chose to orient toward the bar side. *P < 0.05/7, **P < 0.01/7, ***P < 0.001/7, one-sample t-test with Bonferroni correction. n.s., P > 0.05/3, ##P < 0.01/3, two-sample t-test with Bonferroni correction. Exact P values for all statistical tests can be found in Supplementary Table 1.

Supplementary Figure 3 The structure of the BU is stereotyped across animals.

(a) In vitro images of the BU in the right hemisphere of two brains. Images show the immunofluorescence of GFP in brains expressing GFP pan-neuronally. A confocal stack that covers the BU (48 frames, 24 μm) was divided into 8 subvolumes (6 frames, 3 μm). Each image is a projection of images in a subvolume. SBU, superior BU; IBU, inferior BU; ABU, anterior BU. MB, mushroom body; FB, fan-shaped body. Scale bar, 10 μm. (b) In vivo images of the BU in two flies expressing GCaMP6f pan-neuronally. A series of GCaMP6f images covering the BU (60 frames, 30 μm) was divided into 10 subvolumes (6 frames, 3 μm). Frames in a subvolume are averaged and normalized to obtain each image. Scale bar, 10 μm. (c) In vivo images of the recorded part of the BU in all flies used for calcium imaging experiments. Each image is obtained from a single fly by averaging 40 frames immediately before cue presentation in a trial. Images are grouped according to the depth of the target volume (see Fig. 3f for the location of target volumes). Scale bar, 10 μm.

Supplementary Figure 4 Activity of individual microglomeruli for each cue-location.

(a–c) Each row represents the time course of the average ΔF/F of one microglomerulus. (a) Individual microglomeruli of the ipsi-preferring population (N = 107 microglomeruli in 21 flies). Microglomeruli are sorted according to the trial-type preference. (b) Same as in a but for the no-preference population (N = 361 microglomeruli in 40 flies). (c) Same as in a but for the contra-preferring population (N = 98 microglomeruli in 17 flies). (d) Population-averaged activity for each cue-location (mean ± s.e.m. across microglomeruli) for the the no-preference population. (e) Distribution of mean ΔF/F of each microglomerulus during the cue period. Box plots indicate median, quartiles, and range. **P < 0.01/2, ***P < 0.001/2, one-sample t-test with Bonferroni correction, the denominator represents the number of comparisons. ###P < 0.001, paired t-test. Exact P values for all statistical tests can be found in Supplementary Table 1.

Supplementary Figure 5 Construction and assessment of a linear regression analysis.

A linear regression analysis of the activity of each microglomerulus was performed after taking into account the delay between calcium signals and intended turns using cross-correlation (see Online Methods). Cross-correlation between ΔF/F and ΔWBA was calculated separately for each task period (mean ± s.e.m. across microglomeruli) for the ipsi-preferring population (a) (N = 107 microglomeruli in 21 flies), the no-preference population (b) (N = 361 microglomeruli in 40 flies), and the contra-preferring population (c) (N = 98 microglomeruli in 17 flies). Positive correlations indicate higher activity for contralateral turns. Negative time lags represent the situations where changes in ΔWBA precede those in ΔF/F. (d) Cross-correlation calculated based on the data from the entire task period for the contra-preferring population (mean ± s.e.m. across microglomeruli). (e) Histogram of the time lag at the peak of cross-correlation, calculated from the entire task period, for each microglomerulus of the contra-preferring population. Red triangles, median. This time lag was considered in the linear regression fit (see Online Methods). (f) Histogram of the quality of the linear regression fit quantified using R-squared. Red triangles, mean.

Supplementary Figure 6 Regression parameters for individual microglomeruli.

Time course of the Y-intercept (a,c,e) and the slope (b,d,f) in individual microglomeruli for each cue-location. Microglomeruli are sorted according to the trial-type preference in each population. (a,b) The ipsi-preferring population (N = 107 microglomeruli in 21 flies). (c,d) The no-preference population (N = 361 microglomeruli in 40 flies). (e,f) The contra-preferring population (N = 98 microglomeruli in 17 flies). (g,h) Time course of regression parameters (mean ± s.e.m. across microglomeruli) for the no-preference population. (i) Distribution of the average Y-intercept of each microglomerulus during the cue period, plotted separately for each cue-location and population. Box plots indicate median, quartiles, and range. ***P < 0.001/2, one-sample t-test with Bonferroni correction, the denominator represents the number of comparisons. ###P < 0.001, paired t-test. (j) Distribution of the difference in the average Y-intercepts between cue-locations during the cue period. Box plots indicate median, quartiles, and range. ***P < 0.001, one-way ANOVA followed by post hoc Tukey’s HSD test. (k) Relationship between the average slope and the Y-intercept difference between cue-locations during the cue period for the contra-preferring population (Pearson’s correlation coefficient, 0.41, P < 0.001). Each dot represents a microlomgerulus. Exact P values for all statistical tests can be found in Supplementary Table 1.

Supplementary Figure 7 Slow changes in ΔWBA drive the turn-related component.

(a) Distribution of turn scores calculated based on ΔWBA, the angle of ipsilateral wing, and that of contralateral wing for the ipsi-preferring population (N = 107 microglomeruli in 21 flies). Turn scores quantify the correlation between an aspect of wingbeat movement (e.g., ΔWBA) and the calcium signals corrected for the effect of cue-location (see Online Methods). Box plots indicate median, quartiles, and range. (b) Same as in a but for the no-preference population (N = 361 microglomeruli in 40 flies). (c) Same as in a but for the contra-preferring population (N = 98 microglomeruli in 17 flies). Turn scores based on ΔWBA were higher than those based on the angle of respective wings. (d) Raw ΔWBA traces were low-pass filtered to separate the low-frequency component (LFC) from the high-frequency component (Raw − LFC). (e) Same as in a but for ΔWBA, the LFC of ΔWBA, and the Raw − LFC. (f) Same as in e but for the no-preference population. (g) Same as in e but for the contra-preferring population. Turn scores based on ΔWBA and the LFC were higher than those for the Raw − LFC. *P < 0.05/3, ***P < 0.001/3, one-sample t-test with Bonferroni correction, the denominator represents the number of comparisons. #P < 0.05, ###P < 0.001, one-way ANOVA followed by post hoc Tukey’s HSD test. Exact P values for all statistical tests can be found in Supplementary Table 1.

Supplementary Figure 8 Distribution of the ipsi-preferring and the contra-preferring populations.

(a) Four example microglomeruli located in the dBU. Images were generated by averaging frames of highest intensity (top 5%) for each microglomerulus. Traces indicate the time course of calcium signals for each cue-location (mean ± s.e.m. across trials). Scale bar, 10 μm. (b) Same as in a but for four other examples located in the vBU. (c) Density of each population in the dBU and the vBU. Images were generated based on the contours of microglomeruli in each recording (e.g., left panel in Fig. 3e). No image registration was performed across brains because we recorded from highly consistent areas across brains (Supplementary Fig. 3c). Scale bar, 10 μm.

Supplementary Figure 9 Microglomeruli in the dBU showing stronger cue responses also show stronger delay-period activity.

(a) Population-averaged activity for each cue-location for the ipsi-preferring population in the dBU (N = 98 microglomeruli in 19 flies; mean ± s.e.m. across microglomeruli). (b) Same as in a but for the no-preference population in the dBU (N = 220 microglomeruli in 20 flies). (c) Distributions of the trial-type preference calculated based on ΔF/F in the last 3 s of the delay period, which quantifies the selectivity for cue-location in this period of activity (see Online Methods). Box plots indicate median, quartiles, and range. The mean trial-type preference in the delay period for the ipsi-preferring population was -0.18 ± 0.02 (mean ± s.e.m., N = 98 microglomeruli; corresponding to the percentage of correct discrimination of 59.0 ± 1.1%), which was more reliable than the choice bias of the flies during the recording (the percentage of uncued choice, 56.3 ± 1.5%, mean ± s.e.m., N = 20 flies). Note that the ordinate is reversed so that values representing higher activity in the ipsilateral cue trials are plotted in the upper part. *P < 0.05, ***P < 0.001, one-sample t-test. ###P < 0.001, two-sample t-test. (d) Relationship between the trial-type preferences calculated based on ΔF/F in the cue period and those calculated based on ΔF/F in the last 3 s of the delay period (Pearson’s correlation coefficient, 0.38, P < 0.001). Each dot represents a microglomerulus (blue, the ipsi-preferring population; gray, the no-preference population). Black lines indicate the population summary of the trial-type preference in the last 3 s of the delay period (mean ± s.e.m. across microglomeruli) calculated every 0.2 of the trial-type preference in the cue period. Exact p-values for all statistical tests can be found in Supplementary Table 1.

Supplementary Figure 10 Delay-period activity in the dBU is composed of calcium transients.

(a) Analyses were based on the data in the last 3 s of the delay period (red). (b) Time course of calcium signals in an ipsilateral cue trial from the example microglomerulus shown in Fig. 3g,h. Calcium signals were filtered with a boxcar filter of 5 frames (255 ms). (c) Detection of calcium transients. Left, magnified view of the trace in b. Right, time derivative of the calcium signal. Arrows indicate the onset of calcium transients, defined as the time the derivative exceeds a threshold (1 ΔF/F/s, dotted line in the right panel). (d) Distribution of the mean number of transients per trial plotted separately for each cue and population in the dBU (the ipsi-preferring population, N = 98 microglomeruli in 19 flies; the no-preference population, N = 220 microglomeruli in 20 flies). Box plots indicate median, quartiles, and range. **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001, paired t-test. (e) Same as in d but for the difference in the mean number of transients between ipsilateral and contralateral cue trials. *P < 0.05, two-sample t-test. (f–s) Characteristics of calcium transients during the delay period for the ipsi-preferring population (f,h–m) and the no-preference population (g,n–s). (f,g) Time course of transients averaged across trials (mean ± s.e.m.). Analyses were done on trials with just one transient occurring in the last 3-s of the delay period with its onset in the first 1 s of the analyzed period (N = 179 ipsilateral and 150 contralateral cue trials for the ipsi-preferring population, N = 284 ipsilateral and 284 contralateral cue trials for the no-preference population). The same sets of trials were used in k–m,q–s. (h,i,n,o) Histogram of the number of transients (h,n) and onset timing (i,o) (N = 1401 ipsilateral and 1399 contralateral cue trials for the ipsi-preferring population, N = 2888 ipsilateral and 2901 contralateral cue trials for the no-preference population). (j,p) Histogram of inter-transient interval calculated from trials with more than one transients (N = 297 ipsilateral and 245 contralateral cue trials for the ipsi-preferring population, N = 438 ipsilateral and 421 contralateral cue trials for the no-preference population). (k,q) Histogram of the peak timing from transient onset. (l,r) Histogram of the peak amplitude. (m,s) Histogram of transient duration (see Online Methods for the definition of the duration). Exact P values for all statistical tests can be found in Supplementary Table 1.

Supplementary Figure 11 Delay-period activity in the dBU may contain persistent as well as transient components.

(a–d) The ipsi-preferring population (N = 98 microglomeruli in 19 flies). (a) Time course of calcium signals for each cue-location plotted separately for the number of calcium transients in the last 3 s of the delay period. The number of trials is indicated in b. (b) Distributions of responses in the cue period for each cue-location and the number of transients per trial. Responses were defined as the difference in mean ΔF/F between the 2-s pre-cue period and the cue period. Box plots indicate median, quartiles, and range. Numbers above the box plots indicate the number of trials in each condition. Stronger cue responses were followed by a larger number of transients in ipsilateral cue trials (Pearson’s correlation coefficient, 0.19, P < 0.001) whereas by a slightly lower number of transients in contralateral cue trials (Pearson’s correlation coefficient, -0.065, P = 0.015). (c) Same as in b but for responses in the last 3 s of the delay period. Responses in the delay period increased with the number of transients per trial (Pearson’s correlation coefficient; ipsilateral cue trials, 0.24, P < 0.001; contralateral cue trials, 0.18, P < 0.001). *P < 0.05/3, **P < 0.01/3, ***P < 0.001/3, paired t-test with Bonferroni correction, the denominator represents the number of comparisons. Tests were performed when the number of trials exceeded 100 trials for both cue-locations. (d) Same as in c but for a lower threshold for defining transients (see Online Methods). Data are shown for conditions where the number of trials exceeded 100 for both cues. **P < 0.01/4, ***P < 0.001/4. (e–h) Same as in a–d but for the no-preference population (N = 220 microglomeruli in 20 flies). (f) Stronger cue responses were followed by a minutely larger number of transients per trial in ipsilateral cue trials (Pearson’s correlation coefficient, 0.037, P = 0.049) and by a lower number of transients in contralateral cue trials (Pearson’s correlation coefficient, -0.11, P < 0.001). (g) Responses in the delay period increased with the number of transients per trial (Pearson’s correlation coefficient; ipsilateral cue trials, 0.18, P < 0.001; contralateral cue trials, 0.15, P < 0.001). **P < 0.01/3, ***P < 0.001/3, paired t-test with Bonferroni correction. (h) * P < 0.05/5, ***P < 0.001/5, paired t-test with Bonferroni correction. Exact P values for all statistical tests can be found in Supplementary Table 1.

Supplementary Figure 12 Localization of PA-GFP photolabeling.

(a) Photoactivation of a volume of the BU at one of the five depths along the dorsal-ventral axis (3 brains for each depth; see Fig. 6b for the location of target volumes). Each image is a projection of a confocal stack that covers the BU. No photoactivation was applied to the control brain. Top: schematic of the imaged region of the brain. Scale bar, 20 μm. (b) Whole-brain photolabeling patterns of brains shown in Fig. 6. Each image is a projection of a confocal stack covering the brain. Top: schematic of the fly brain. Scale bar, 100 μm. (c) Photolabeling in the AOTU of a representative brain. Images are projections of a confocal stack covering the AOTU. The AOTU is composed of medial, intermediate, and lateral zones (top image). Top: schematic of the imaged region of the brain. Magenta, nc82; green, PA-GFP. Scale bar, 20 μm.

Supplementary Figure 13 Photolabeling in other PA-GFP experiments.

(a,b) Photolabeling in the EB (a) and the AOTU (b) in brains not shown in Fig. 6 in which a volume of the BU was photoactivated at one of the five depths along the dorsal-ventral axis (2 brains for each depth; see Fig. 6b for the location of target volumes). Each image is a projection of a confocal stack covering a subvolume of the AOTU (a) or the EB (b) as in Fig. 6d,e. Top: regions of the target brain structures in a standard brain. Scale bars, 20 μm. (c–f) Photoactivation of the ventromedial (VM) or the ventrolateral (VL) parts of the BU. (c) Schematic of photoactivated volumes (red). (d) Photolabeling in the BU for VL and VM photoactivation (3 brains for each volume). Each image is a projection of a confocal stack covering the BU. Top: schematic of the imaged region of the brain. Scale bars, 20 μm. (e,f) Same as in a,b but for VL and VM photoactivation.

Supplementary Figure 14 Different types of ring neurons innervate different parts of the BU.

(a) Gal4-expression patterns in the BU of the right hemisphere. Images show the immunofluorescence of GFP (green) expressed using a pan-neuronal driver line and the red fluorescent protein (RFP, orange) expressed using driver lines specific to type(s) of ring neurons. Each image is a projection of a confocal stack that covers a subvolume of the BU (6 frames, 3 μm). Scale bar, 10 μm. Bottom panels show schematics of the region of the BU labeled. c42-Gal4 labels R2 and R4 neurons34, R38H02-Gal4 labels R4 neurons11, R15B07-Gal4 labels R1 and R4 neurons11, c232-Gal4 labels R3 and R4d neurons34, R64H04-Gal4 labels R3 neurons, and R28D01-Gal4 labels R1 neurons11. R28D01-Gal4 also labeled microglomeruli in the dBU weakly, which was visible with a strong laser (the rightmost column). The labeling of some of these microglomeruli extended to the AOTU (data not shown). (b) Gal4-expression patterns in the EB. Each image is a projection of a confocal stack covering the EB. Scale bar, 20 μm.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Text and Figures

Supplementary Figures 1–14 and Supplementary Table 1. (PDF 3283 kb)

Life Sciences Reporting Summary (PDF 158 kb)

Supplementary Table 2

Details of statistical tests. (XLSX 18 kb)

Cueing biases flight orientation

Two example trials of the cueing experiment (Fig. 1d) in which the example fly shown in Fig. 1e chose the bar at the uncued location. Top left: schematic of the visual pattern presented in the arena subtending the visual angle of 330° in azimuth, projected to a rectangle. The fly is facing toward the center of the rectangle. Bottom left: a bottom view of the tethered fly. Images are flipped horizontally so that the left wing is displayed on the left side. Green lines indicate the estimates of the edges of the wings. Top right: the trajectory of the fly during the choice period. The red triangle indicates the location of the cue. (MP4 4433 kb)

A microglomerulus in the BU showing reactivation during the delay period

A trial of the pan-neuronal calcium imaging experiment in which the example microglomerulus shown in Fig. 3g was activated by the ipsilateral cue presentation. This microglomerulus was reactivated during the delay period. Top left: schematic of the visual pattern. The same format as in Supplementary Video 1. Bottom left: a bottom view of the tethered fly. The same format as in Supplementary Video 1. Top right: time course of the calcium response of the microglomerulus (red) and ΔWBA (black). Bottom right: imaging frames from which the response of the microglomerulus was extracted. The white line denotes the boundary of the microglomerulus. Each image was smoothed by a Gaussian filter (s.d., 1 pixel). Size of frames, 30.2 × 30.2 μm. (MP4 2443 kb)

A microglomerulus in the BU showing intended-turn-related activity

A trial of the pan-neuronal calcium imaging experiment in which the example microglomerulus shown in Fig. 3i was inhibited by the ipsilateral cue presentation. Activity of this microglomerulus was highly correlated with ΔWBA. The same format as in Supplementary Video 2. (MP4 2534 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Shiozaki, H., Kazama, H. Parallel encoding of recent visual experience and self-motion during navigation in Drosophila. Nat Neurosci 20, 1395–1403 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4628

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4628

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing