Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Neural computations of threat in the aftermath of combat trauma

An Author Correction to this article was published on 13 February 2019

This article has been updated

Abstract

By combining computational, morphological, and functional analyses, this study relates latent markers of associative threat learning to overt post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms in combat veterans. Using reversal learning, we found that symptomatic veterans showed greater physiological adjustment to cues that did not predict what they had expected, indicating greater sensitivity to prediction errors for negative outcomes. This exaggerated weighting of prediction errors shapes the dynamic learning rate (associability) and value of threat predictive cues. The degree to which the striatum tracked the associability partially mediated the positive correlation between prediction-error weights and PTSD symptoms, suggesting that both increased prediction-error weights and decreased striatal tracking of associability independently contribute to PTSD symptoms. Furthermore, decreased neural tracking of value in the amygdala, in addition to smaller amygdala volume, independently corresponded to higher PTSD symptom severity. These results provide evidence for distinct neurocomputational contributions to PTSD symptoms.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Rent or buy this article

Prices vary by article type

from$1.95

to$39.95

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Experimental overview
Fig. 2: Computational model comparison and relationship to PTSD symptoms.
Fig. 3: Amygdala structure and value computation contribute to PTSD symptoms using a hybrid computational model of associability and value encoding.
Fig. 4: Neural computations of value, associability and prediction error and their relationship to CAPS symptoms for different ROIs.
Fig. 5: Associability-related neural activity in the right striatum partially mediates the relationship between prediction-error weights and CAPS.

Similar content being viewed by others

Code availability

The code used for the analyses is available online at: http://osf.io/rxsw2/.

Data availability

Data used to support the conclusions of this study is available online at: http://osf.io/rxsw2/.

Change history

  • 13 February 2019

    The original and corrected figures are shown in the accompanying Author Correction.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn (American Psychiatric Publishing, Arlington, 2013).

  2. Pietrzak, R. H., Tsai, J., Harpaz-Rotem, I., Whealin, J. M. & Southwick, S. M. Support for a novel five-factor model of posttraumatic stress symptoms in three independent samples of Iraq/Afghanistan veterans: a confirmatory factor analytic study. J. Psychiatr. Res. 46, 317–322 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Harpaz-Rotem, I., Tsai, J., Pietrzak, R. H. & Hoff, R. The dimensional structure of posttraumatic stress symptomatology in 323,903 U.S. veterans. J. Psychiatr. Res. 49, 31–36 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Lissek, S. & van Meurs, B. Learning models of PTSD: theoretical accounts and psychobiological evidence. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 98, 594–605 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Pavlov, I. Conditioned Reflexes (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 1927).

  6. Pearce, J. M. & Hall, G. A model for Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychol. Rev. 87, 532–552 (1980).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Schiller, D., Levy, I., Niv, Y., LeDoux, J. E. & Phelps, E. A. From fear to safety and back: reversal of fear in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 28, 11517–11525 (2008).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Li, J., Schiller, D., Schoenbaum, G., Phelps, E. A. & Daw, N. D. Differential roles of human striatum and amygdala in associative learning. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 1250–1252 (2011).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Atlas, L. Y., Doll, B. B., Li, J., Daw, N. D. & Phelps, E. A. Instructed knowledge shapes feedback-driven aversive learning in striatum and orbitofrontal cortex, but not the amygdala. eLife 5, e15192 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Duits, P. et al. Updated meta-analysis of classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders. Depress. Anxiety 32, 239–253 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Browning, M., Behrens, T. E., Jocham, G., O’Reilly, J. X. & Bishop, S. J. Anxious individuals have difficulty learning the causal statistics of aversive environments. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 590–596 (2015).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. LeDoux, J. E. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 155–184 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Pietrzak, R. H. et al. Amygdala-hippocampal volume and the phenotypic heterogeneity of posttraumatic stress disorder: a cross-sectional study. JAMA Psychiatry 72, 396–398 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Admon, R. et al. Human vulnerability to stress depends on amygdala’s predisposition and hippocampal plasticity. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 106, 14120–14125 (2009).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Neumeister, P. et al. Specific amygdala response to masked fearful faces in post-traumatic stress relative to other anxiety disorders. Psychol. Med. 48, 1209–1217 (2018).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Rescorla, R. & Wagner, A. in Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory (eds Black A. H. & Prokasy, W. F.) (Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, 1972).

  17. Roesch, M. R., Esber, G. R., Li, J., Daw, N. D. & Schoenbaum, G. Surprise! Neural correlates of Pearce–Hall and Rescorla–Wagner coexist within the brain. Eur. J. Neurosci. 35, 1190–1200 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Jin, J., Zelano, C., Gottfried, J. A. & Mohanty, A. Human amygdala represents the complete spectrum of subjective valence. J. Neurosci. 35, 15145–15156 (2015).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Belova, M. A., Paton, J. J., Morrison, S. E. & Salzman, C. D. Expectation modulates neural responses to pleasant and aversive stimuli in primate amygdala. Neuron 55, 970–984 (2007).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Klavir, O., Genud-Gabai, R. & Paz, R. Functional connectivity between amygdala and cingulate cortex for adaptive aversive learning. Neuron 80, 1290–1300 (2013).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Genud-Gabai, R., Klavir, O. & Paz, R. Safety signals in the primate amygdala. J. Neurosci. 33, 17986–17994 (2013).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Morey, R. A. et al. Mid-Atlantic MIRECC Workgroup. Amygdala volume changes in posttraumatic stress disorder in a large case-controlled veterans group. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69, 1169–1178 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Wrocklage, K. M. et al. Cortical thickness reduction in combat exposed U.S. veterans with and without PTSD. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 27, 515–525 (2017).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Raio, C. M., Hartley, C. A., Orederu, T. A., Li, J. & Phelps, E. A. Stress attenuates the flexible updating of aversive value. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 11241–11246 (2017).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Roesch, M. R., Calu, D. J., Esber, G. R. & Schoenbaum, G. Neural correlates of variations in event processing during learning in basolateral amygdala. J. Neurosci. 30, 2464–2471 (2010).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Schultz, W. Dopamine neurons and their role in reward mechanisms. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 7, 191–197 (1997).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. O’Doherty, J. P., Dayan, P., Friston, K., Critchley, H. & Dolan, R. J. Temporal difference models and reward-related learning in the human brain. Neuron 38, 329–337 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Preuschoff, K. & Bossaerts, P. Adding prediction risk to the theory of reward learning. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1104, 135–146 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Behrens, T. E., Woolrich, M. W., Walton, M. E. & Rushworth, M. F. Learning the value of information in an uncertain world. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 1214–1221 (2007).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Etkin, A. & Wager, T. D. Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: a meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. Am. J. Psychiatry 164, 1476–1488 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Ruderman, L. et al. Posttraumatic symptoms and aversion to ambiguous losses in combat veterans. Depress. Anxiety 33, 606–613 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Brown, V. M. et al. Associability-modulated loss learning is increased in post-traumatic stress disorder. eLife 7, e30150 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Daw, N. D. in Decision Making, Affect, and Learning: Attention and Performance XXIII (eds Delgado, M. R., Phelps, E. A. & Robbins, T. W.) (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 2011).

  34. Spiegelhalter, D., Best, N., Carlin, B. & Van der Linde, A. Bayesian measures of model complexity and fit (with discussion). J. R. Stat. Soc. B 64, 583–639 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Gelman, A. et al. Bayesian Data Analysis 3rd edn (CRC Press, Boca Raton, 2013).

  36. Myung, I. J. Tutorial on maximum likelihood estimation. J. Math. Psychol. 47, 90–100 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Ahn, W.-Y., Haines, N. & Zhang, L. Revealing neurocomputational mechanisms of reinforcement learning and decision-making with the hbayesdm package. Comput. Psychiatr. 1, 24–57 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Gelman, A., Lee, D. & Guo, J. Stan: A probabilistic programming language for Bayesian inference and optimization. J. Educ. Behav. Stat. 40, 530–543 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Ahn, W.-Y. et al. Decision-making in stimulant and opiate addicts in protracted abstinence: evidence from computational modeling with pure users. Front. Psychol. 5, 849 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Gelman, A. Prior distributions for variance parameters in hierarchical models (comment on article by Browne and Draper). Bayesian. Anal. 1, 515–534 (2006).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Gelman, A. & Rubin, D. B. Inference from iterative simulation using multiple sequences. Stat. Sci. 7, 457–472 (1992).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Savalia, N. K. et al. Motion-related artifacts in structural brain images revealed with independent estimates of in-scanner head motion. Hum. Brain. Mapp. 38, 472–492 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Cools, R., Clark, L., Owen, A. M. & Robbins, T. W. Defining the neural mechanisms of probabilistic reversal learning using event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. 22, 4563–4567 (2002).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Dodds, C. M. et al. Methylphenidate has differential effects on blood oxygenation level-dependent signal related to cognitive subprocesses of reversal learning. J. Neurosci. 28, 5976–5982 (2008).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  45. Maldjian, J. A., Laurienti, P. J., Kraft, R. A. & Burdette, J. H. An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. Neuroimage 19, 1233–1239 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The main source of funding for this work was provided by NIMH 105535 R01 grant awarded to I. Harpaz-Rotem and D. Schiller (MPI) and funding provided by the Clinical Neurosciences Division of the National Center for PTSD. Additional support was provided by Klingenstein-Simons Fellowship Award in the Neurosciences to D. Schiller, The Brain and Behavior Research Foundation 23260 to I. Harpaz-Rotem, Chinese NSF grant 31421003 to J. Li, and the Swiss National Science Foundation grant SNF 161077 to P. Homan. The analytic work was supported in part through the computational resources and staff expertise provided by Scientific Computing at the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

I.L., I.H.R., and D.S. designed the study. E.F., C.G., I.L., and I.H.R. collected the data. J.H. scored the data. P.H. analyzed the data. I.L., J.L, I.H.R., and D.S. contributed to data analysis. J.H.K., R.H.P., and S.S. contributed to the interpretation of the results. P.H., I.L., I.H.R., and D.S. wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the final version of the manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Ilan Harpaz-Rotem or Daniela Schiller.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors report no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Integrated supplementary information

Supplementary Figure 1 Distributions of psychopathology in the study sample show the wide range of symptoms in participants, from completely healthy to highly affected by PTSD.

ASI, Anxiety Sensitivity Index; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; CAPS, Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale; CES, combat exposure score; STAIS, State Anxiety subscale of the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory.

Supplementary Figure 2 Hierarchical Bayesian model recovers simulated learning parameters.

To validate our Hierarchical Bayesian hybrid model before testing it in the clinical dataset, we simulated skin conductance data in 20 participants. We estimated the initial associability α0, κ, the normalization factor, and η, the prediction-error weight. a. Simulated data in 20 subjects. b. Correlations between estimated and simulated values. The Bayesian model successfully recovered the simulated values. Pearson correlation coefficients with two-tailed significance tests are shown. Error shadings correspond to standard errors. c. Estimated parameters with 90% credible intervals and simulated parameters. All simulated parameters were within the 90% credible intervals of the estimated parameters, indicating successful parameter recovery. ***, P < 0.001.

Supplementary Figure 3 Average skin conductance response across subjects and the best-fit associability trace for all trials without a shock.

As there were two experimental orders, time courses are displayed for each order separately (N = 54 participants). All traces were z-transformed and mean-centered for displaying purpose.

Supplementary Figure 4 Model comparison using maximum likelihood estimation (MLE) and the relationship of model fits to PTSD symptoms.

a. MLE analysis is consistent with the Bayesian analysis and a previous study. In line with a previous study,8 the hybrid model with the lowest Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) was the hybrid (α + V) model, outperforming the other hybrid models as well as the Rescorla–Wagner (RW) model. The asterisk indicates the winning model. b. No evidence that model fits interacted with the symptom levels of PTSD. The correlation between model fits and symptoms was essentially flat for each of the 4 models. Error shadings correspond to standard errors. PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder. CAPS, Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale; RW, Rescorla–Wagner model.

Supplementary Figure 5 Hierarchical Bayesian model recovers simulated learning parameters of a Rescorla–Wagner model.

To validate our Hierarchical Bayesian model before testing it in the clinical dataset, we simulated skin conductance data in 30 participants. We estimated the learning rate α (the free parameter in the Rescorla–Wagner model) using our Hierarchical Bayesian model coded in Stan and also compared it to a Maximum Likelihood Estimation (MLE) using the non-linear optimizer fmincon in MATLAB. The Bayesian model successfully recovered the simulated values.

Supplementary Figure 6 Average skin conductance response across subjects and the best-fit expected value trace for all trials without a shock using a Rescorla-Wagner model.

As there were two experimental orders, time courses are displayed for each order separately (N = 54 participants). All traces were z-transformed and mean-centered for displaying purpose.

Supplementary Figure 7 Amygdala structure and value computation contribute to PTSD symptoms using a Rescorla–Wagner model.

a. Regions of interest used in the computational imaging analysis. The amygdala (red) was defined functionally, using the contrast of conditioned stimulus vs. baseline. b-d. Amygdala volume and value-dependent neural activity independently contribute to PTSD symptoms. Partial correlations are shown (N = 54 participants) with Pearson correlation coefficients and two-tailed significance tests. Right amygdala volume and activity as well as left amygdala activity correlated negatively with the PTSD symptoms as measured with the CAPS. Regressions were adjusted for learning rate, age, gender, head movement, and total intracranial volume. Error shadings correspond to standard errors. CAPS, Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale; adj., adjusted; **, P < 0.01; *, P < 0.05.

Supplementary Figure 8 Individual amygdala regions of interest for two illustrative participants.

To ensure accurate appearance in the published version, please use the Symbol font for all symbols and Greek letters.Brain slices are shown at x = 24, y = −4, z = −16 in the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinate system. using the neurological convention (that is, left is left).

Supplementary Figure 9 Regression of prediction error (δ) and associability (α) shows that the two learning components were not significantly correlated.

Data for both experimental orders (A and B) is shown (N = 69 trials) together with Pearson correlation coefficients and two-tailed significance tests. Error shadings correspond to standard errors.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Text and Figures

Supplementary Figures 1–9 and Supplementary Table 1

Reporting Summary

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Homan, P., Levy, I., Feltham, E. et al. Neural computations of threat in the aftermath of combat trauma. Nat Neurosci 22, 470–476 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0315-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0315-x

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing