Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Neural circuits for evidence accumulation and decision making in larval zebrafish

Abstract

To make appropriate decisions, animals need to accumulate sensory evidence. Simple integrator models can explain many aspects of such behavior, but how the underlying computations are mechanistically implemented in the brain remains poorly understood. Here we approach this problem by adapting the random-dot motion discrimination paradigm, classically used in primate studies, to larval zebrafish. Using their innate optomotor response as a measure of decision making, we find that larval zebrafish accumulate and remember motion evidence over many seconds and that the behavior is in close agreement with a bounded leaky integrator model. Through the use of brain-wide functional imaging, we identify three neuronal clusters in the anterior hindbrain that are well suited to execute the underlying computations. By relating the dynamics within these structures to individual behavioral choices, we propose a biophysically plausible circuit arrangement in which an evidence integrator competes against a dynamic decision threshold to activate a downstream motor command.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Behavior and modeling in freely swimming larval zebrafish.
Fig. 2: Behavior and modeling in head-fixed larval zebrafish.
Fig. 3: Brain-wide and anterior hindbrain two-photon calcium imaging.
Fig. 4: Neural correlates of behavioral choices in the anterior hindbrain.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.

Code availability

All analysis and hardware control code will be made available by the corresponding author upon request.

References

  1. Shadlen, M. N. & Kiani, R. Decision making as a window on cognition. Neuron 80, 791–806 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Ratcliff, R., Smith, P. L., Brown, S. D. & McKoon, G. Diffusion decision model: current issues and history. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 260–281 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Groschner, L. N., Chan Wah Hak, L., Bogacz, R., DasGupta, S. & Miesenböck, G. Dendritic integration of sensory evidence in perceptual decision-making. Cell 173, 894–905 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Borst, A. & Bahde, S. Visual information processing in the fly’s landing system. J. Comp. Physiol. A 163, 167–173 (1988).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. DasGupta, S., Ferreira, C. H. & Miesenböck, G. FoxP influences the speed and accuracy of a perceptual decision in Drosophila. Science 344, 901–904 (2014).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Evans, D. A. et al. A synaptic threshold mechanism for computing escape decisions. Nature 558, 590–594 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Odoemene, O., Pisupati, S., Nguyen, H. & Churchland, A. K. Visual evidence accumulation guides decision-making in unrestrained mice. J. Neurosci. 38, 10143–10155 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Hanks, T. D. et al. Distinct relationships of parietal and prefrontal cortices to evidence accumulation. Nature 520, 220–223 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Hanks, T. D., Ditterich, J. & Shadlen, M. N. Microstimulation of macaque area LIP affects decision-making in a motion discrimination task. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 682–689 (2006).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Huk, A. C., Katz, L. N. & Yates, J. L. The role of the lateral intraparietal area in (the study of) decision making. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 40, 349–372 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Shadlen, M. N. & Newsome, W. T. Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 86, 1916–1936 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Roitman, J. D. & Shadlen, M. N. Response of neurons in the lateral intraparietal area during a combined visual discrimination reaction time task. J. Neurosci. 22, 9475–9489 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Newsome, W. T., Britten, K. H. & Movshon, J. A. Neuronal correlates of a perceptual decision. Nature 341, 52–54 (1989).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Newsome, W. T. & Pare, E. B. A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT). J. Neurosci. 8, 2201–2211 (1988).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Salzman, C. D., Britten, K. H. & Newsome, W. T. Cortical microstimulation influences perceptual judgements of motion direction. Nature 346, 174–177 (1990).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Naumann, E. A. et al. From whole-brain data to functional circuit models: the zebrafish optomotor response. Cell 167, 947–960 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Portugues, R., Haesemeyer, M., Blum, M. L. & Engert, F. Whole-field visual motion drives swimming in larval zebrafish via a stochastic process. J. Exp. Biol. 218, 1433–1443 (2015).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Ahrens, M. B. et al. Brain-wide neuronal dynamics during motor adaptation in zebrafish. Nature 485, 471–477 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Kubo, F. et al. Functional architecture of an optic flow-responsive area that drives horizontal eye movements in zebrafish. Neuron 81, 1344–1359 (2014).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Wang, K., Hinz, J., Haikala, V., Reiff, D. F. & Arrenberg, A. B. Selective processing of all rotational and translational optic flow directions in the zebrafish pretectum and tectum. BMC Biol. 17, 29 (2019).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Dunn, T. W. et al. Brain-wide mapping of neural activity controlling zebrafish exploratory locomotion. Elife 5, e12741 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Chen, X. et al. Brain-wide organization of neuronal activity and convergent sensorimotor transformations in larval zebrafish. Neuron 100, 876–890 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Pnevmatikakis, E. A. et al. Simultaneous denoising, deconvolution, and demixing of calcium imaging data. Neuron 89, 285–299 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Randlett, O. et al. Whole-brain activity mapping onto a zebrafish brain atlas. Nat. Methods 12, 1039–1046 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Satou, C. et al. Transgenic tools to characterize neuronal properties of discrete populations of zebrafish neurons. Development 140, 3927–3931 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Kunst, M. et al. A cellular-resolution atlas of the larval zebrafish brain. Neuron 103, 21–38 (2019).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Huang, K.-H., Ahrens, M. B., Dunn, T. W. & Engert, F. Spinal projection neurons control turning behaviors in zebrafish. Curr. Biol. 23, 1566–1573 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Orger, M. B., Kampff, A. R., Severi, K. E., Bollmann, J. H. & Engert, F. Control of visually guided behavior by distinct populations of spinal projection neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 327–333 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Fisher, D., Olasagasti, I., Tank, D. W., Aksay, E. R. F. & Goldman, M. S. A modeling framework for deriving the structural and functional architecture of a short-term memory microcircuit. Neuron 79, 987–1000 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Chen, T.-W. et al. Ultrasensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature 499, 295–300 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Ehrlich, D. E. & Schoppik, D. Control of movement initiation underlies the development of balance. Curr. Biol. 27, 334–344 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Oteiza, P., Odstrcil, I., Lauder, G., Portugues, R. & Engert, F. A novel mechanism for mechanosensory-based rheotaxis in larval zebrafish. Nature 547, 445–448 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Orger, M. B., Smear, M. C., Anstis, S. M. & Baier, H. Perception of Fourier and non-Fourier motion by larval zebrafish. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1128–1133 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Wolf, S. et al. Sensorimotor computation underlying phototaxis in zebrafish. Nat. Commun. 8, 1–12 (2017).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Haesemeyer, M., Robson, D. N., Li, J. M., Schier, A. F. & Engert, F. A brain-wide circuit model of heat-evoked swimming behavior in larval zebrafish. Neuron 98, 817–831 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Northmore, D. P. M. Holding visual attention for 400 million years: A model of tectum and torus longitudinalis in teleost fishes. Vision Res. 131, 44–56 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Akrami, A., Kopec, C. D., Diamond, M. E. & Brody, C. D. Posterior parietal cortex represents sensory history and mediates its effects on behaviour. Nature 554, 368–372 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Wang, X.-J. Probabilistic decision making by slow reverberation in cortical circuits. Neuron 36, 955–968 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Zhang, J. The effects of evidence bounds on decision-making: theoretical and empirical developments. Front. Psychol. 3, 1–19 (2012).

    Google Scholar 

  40. Thura, D. & Cisek, P. Modulation of premotor and primary motor cortical activity during volitional adjustments of speed-accuracy trade-offs. J. Neurosci. 36, 938–956 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Hanks, T. D., Mazurek, M. E., Kiani, R., Hopp, E. & Shadlen, M. N. Elapsed decision time affects the weighting of prior probability in a perceptual decision task. J. Neurosci. 31, 6339–6352 (2011).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Drugowitsch, J., Moreno-Bote, R., Churchland, A. K., Shadlen, M. N. & Pouget, A. The cost of accumulating evidence in perceptual decision making. J. Neurosci. 32, 3612–3628 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Goldman, M. S. Memory without feedback in a neural network. Neuron 61, 621–634 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Lim, S. & Goldman, M. S. Balanced cortical microcircuitry for maintaining information in working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 1306–1314 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Grama, A. & Engert, F. Direction selectivity in the larval zebrafish tectum is mediated by asymmetric inhibition. Front. Neural Circuits 6, 59 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Förster, D., Dal Maschio, M., Laurell, E. & Baier, H. An optogenetic toolbox for unbiased discovery of functionally connected cells in neural circuits. Nat. Commun. 8, 116 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Hildebrand, D. G. C. et al. Whole-brain serial-section electron microscopy in larval zebrafish. Nature 545, 345–349 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Dal Maschio, M., Donovan, J. C., Helmbrecht, T. O. & Baier, H. Linking neurons to network function and behavior by two-photon holographic optogenetics and volumetric imaging. Neuron 94, 774–789 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Kim, D. H. et al. Pan-neuronal calcium imaging with cellular resolution in freely swimming zebrafish. Nat. Methods 14, 1107–1114 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Haug, M. F., Biehlmaier, O., Mueller, K. P. & Neuhauss, S. C. Visual acuity in larval zebrafish: behavior and histology. Front. Zool. 7, 1–8 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Rohlfing, T. & Maurer, C. R. Jr. Nonrigid image registration in shared-memory multiprocessor environments with application to brains, breasts, and bees. IEEE Trans. Inf. Technol. Biomed. 7, 16–25 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Pnevmatikakis, E. A. & Giovannucci, A. NoRMCorre: An online algorithm for piecewise rigid motion correction of calcium imaging data. J. Neurosci. Methods 291, 83–94 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Portugues, R., Feierstein, C. E., Engert, F. & Orger, M. B. Whole-brain activity maps reveal stereotyped, distributed networks for visuomotor behavior. Neuron 81, 1328–1343 (2014).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Avants, B. B., Epstein, C. L., Grossman, M. & Gee, J. C. Symmetric diffeomorphic image registration with cross-correlation: evaluating automated labeling of elderly and neurodegenerative brain. Med. Image Anal. 12, 26–41 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Marquart, G. D. et al. High-precision registration between zebrafish brain atlases using symmetric diffeomorphic normalization. Gigascience 6, 1–15 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  56. Ginzburg, I. & Sompolinsky, H. Theory of correlations in stochastic neural networks. Phys. Rev. E. Stat. Phys. Plasmas Fluids Relat. Interdiscip. Topics 50, 3171–3191 (1994).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Choi, S., Kim, T. & Yu, W. Performance evaluation of RANSAC family. Proc. British Machine Vision Conference 2009 1–12 (British Machine Vision Association, 2009)..

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to H. Sompolinsky for discussions, and A. Borst, J. Drugowitsch, M. Haesemeyer, K. Herrera, R. Harpaz, and K. Vogt for discussions and helpful comments on the manuscript. We thank S. Foianini for fish line maintenance and assistance with the behavioral experiments and K. Herrera for help with fish crossings. A.B. was supported by the Human Frontier Science Program Long-Term Fellowship LT000626/2016. F.E. received funding from the National Institutes of Health (U19 NS104653, R43 OD024879, and R24 NS086601), and the Simons Foundation (SCGB 325207 and SCGB 542973).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

A.B. and F.E. conceived the project. A.B. designed and performed the experiments, built and programmed the behavioral setups and the two-photon microscope, analyzed the data, and carried out the modeling. A.B. wrote the manuscript with help from F.E.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Armin Bahl.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Extended data

Extended Data Fig. 1 Behavior in freely swimming larval zebrafish.

a, Turn angle larvae accumulate over time (positive, right; negative, left). b, Probability distribution of inter-bout intervals during presentation of different coherence levels. c, Time-binned inter-bout intervals as a function of time. d, Probability distributions of turn angles per bout relative to motion direction. Bouts are defined as correct when they follow motion direction (positive) and incorrect otherwise (negative). e, Time-binned precision (absolute turn angle) of correct and incorrect bouts over time. Gray shaded areas in a,c,e indicate motion presentation. Before and after, we always show 0% coherence. Bin sizes in c,e are 2 s. All error bars are mean ± s.e.m. over fish. n = 60 fish in ae, same fish as in Fig. 1b–d.

Extended Data Fig. 2 Model alternatives for freely swimming larval zebrafish.

ad, Schematics and simulation results of alternative models. For simplicity, none of these models had visual feedback. Quantification as in Fig. 1b–f,h–l. n = 16 model runs for each model. e, Pearson’s correlation coefficient between each model feature and the respective experimental data. We use the average of these values to rank the models. All error bars in ad are mean ± s.e.m. over model runs.

Extended Data Fig. 3 Behavior and modeling in head-fixed larval zebrafish.

a, 100 randomly selected experimental example trials with periods of coherent motion (shades of gray) and 0% coherence (lightest gray), sorted by response delay. Note that after each correct (green dots) or incorrect (blue dots) bout, coherence levels are immediately set to 0%. b, Probability density distributions of response delays for correct (solid lines) and incorrect (dashed lines) bouts, for experiment (black lines) and models (red, orange, and blue lines). Models have the same structure as in Extended Data Fig. 2a,c,d but without spontaneous bouts below the bound and different parameters. c, Accuracy as a function of response delay (short, 0–2 s; medium, 2–4 s; long, 4–6 s) for experiment (*P < 0.05 for both comparisons at 50% coherence) and models. Behavioral data in ac come from the experiment with constant motion coherence (Fig. 2b). dg, Behavior quantification for experiment and models, as in Fig. 2c,e,g–i,k–m. h, Pearson’s correlation coefficient between each model feature and the respective experimental data. We use the average of these values to rank the models. n = 13 fish in ad, n = 10 fish in e, n = 8 fish (left panels) and n = 6 fish (right panel) in f, n = 13 fish in g, same fish as in Fig. 2c,e,g–i,k–m. n = 8 model runs for each model in bh. All error bars are mean ± s.e.m. over fish. P values in (c) are based on one-sided t-tests comparing response differences to zero. Asterisks in c indicate significance (*P < 0.05).

Extended Data Fig. 4 Detailed quantification of responsive brain areas identified during brain-wide calcium imaging.

a, All brain areas with >1% responsive cells, sorted by fitted onset time constant during 50% coherent motion. Text label colors relate to colors in (b) and Fig. 3c,d. Bar colors represent fraction of responsive cells within a brain region. Black arrows indicate anterior hindbrain regions with slow dynamics and a large fraction of responsive cells. b, Brain areas with >15% responsive cells sorted by temporal dynamics (top, fast; bottom, slow) characterized from left to right. Column 1: Peak-normalized calcium dynamics, relative to baseline (C0) averaged over all cells responding to coherent motion in preferred- (PD) or null-direction (ND), respectively. Column 2: Average (last 5 s of coherent motion) calcium response amplitude (comparisons between 50% and 100%, from top to bottom: P = 0.05, P = 0.06, P = 0.16, P < 0.05, P < 0.01, P < 0.05, P < 0.05). Column 3: Variance (over time), calculated in individual cells and trials, then averaged, during 0% coherence Column 4: Same as column 3 but time-binned for regions preferred- and null-direction. As variances for preferred- and null-direction motion quickly converge after motion stimulation, the last time bin reflects a motion-memory-independent variance at 0% coherence. c, Preferred- and null-direction dynamics of all identified anterior hindbrain cells functionally clustered by regressor analysis (Fig. 3e). Preferred motion direction refers to motion to the left or right for cells in the left or right hemisphere, respectively, null-direction motion the other way around. d, Spatial arrangement of trial-to-trial reliability for all cells without functional clustering, as in Fig. 3g but for all three coherence levels. n = 6 fish for 50% and n = 6 fish for 100% motion coherence stimulation in a,b. Open circles in a,b indicate individual fish. Note that in some fish not all brain areas were imaged and, hence, fish number per brain region is variable. n = 6 fish in c,d. All error bars in a,b indicate mean ± s.e.m. over fish. Shaded gray areas in b and dashed vertical lines in c indicate motion stimulation. Before and after 0% coherence is shown. All P values are based on two-sided t-tests.

Extended Data Fig. 5 Neurotransmitter identity and neuronal morphology in the anterior hindbrain.

a, Overlay of neurotransmitter type-specific masks from the Z-brain atlas (refs. 24,25) (red outline, glutamate; blue outline, GABA) with the functionally characterized cell types (same cells as in Fig. 3f) in the same coordinate system. Note that almost all identified dynamic threshold neurons lie within the Gad1b cluster 2 and are therefore likely to be inhibitory. Please also note that the expression pattern of the vglut2a-driver line (ref. 25) suggests that the anatomical mask of the VGlut2 cluster 1 is likely to extend even further into the anterior part of the hindbrain. b, Simultaneous imaging of DsRed (pink), expressed only in excitatory vglut2a+ neurons (left panel) or of DsRed expressed only in inhibitory gad1b+ neurons (right panel), and cytosolic or nuclear-localized GCaMP6s (green, pan-neuronal expression). Colored ellipses are manually added to highlight regions of interest. c, Single-cell morphologies with somata in the identified regions in the anterior hindbrain. Cells were mapped from the Max-Planck Zebrafish Brain Atlas (ref. 26) into the Z-brain coordinate system and overlaid with the available masks (gray) on a GCaMP5G reference larval zebrafish (green). n = 6 fish in a, same data as in Fig. 3f. n = 1 fish for each plot in b.

Extended Data Fig. 6 Neural correlates of behavioral choices.

a, Measured cluster dynamics aligned to swim bouts (same data as in Fig. 4b, but showing only ipsilateral dynamics for medium response delays). The thick black line illustrates the difference between the dynamic threshold cluster and the evidence integrator cluster, which crosses zero (baseline) around the same time the motor command cluster reaches its maximum. This event occurs slightly after the bout, probably owing to delays introduced by the relatively slow dynamics of the GCaMP6s indicator (ref. 30). The transparent thin lines are single-trial responses. b,c, Same analysis as in Fig. 4b,c but for bout-aligned network model simulations (*P < 0.001 for the integrator and dynamic threshold unit comparisons; P = 0.58 and P = 0.06 for the motor command cluster comparisons). d, Bout-aligned preferred- and null-direction dynamics of all identified cells from all n = 5 fish, functionally clustered by the behavior-based classification method. Preferred motion direction refers to motion to the left or right for cells in the left or right hemisphere, respectively; null-direction motion the other way around. e, Probability density distributions of response delays for correct (solid lines) and incorrect (dashed lines) bouts, for experiment (black) and network model (brown). f, Illustration of two methods for trial-by-trial prediction of individual behavioral choices based on the experimentally obtained cluster dynamics. Bouts are predicted when the smoothed and extrapolated integrator cluster activity (red dashed lines are exponential fits of the experimental data, solid red lines) crosses the threshold (cyan lines). Three fixed thresholds (left panel) and the dynamic threshold (right panel) are tested. The third method, which uses a sudden rise in the motor command slope as a predictor, is not illustrated. g, Quantification of the fraction of trials in which a threshold crossing event is detected and quantification of predictive quality (coefficient of determination, R2) for the different threshold models (*P < 0.05 for each model compared to the dynamic threshold model). Gray lines are individual fish, black lines are fish averages. h, Trial-by-trial predictions of bout-timing for individual fish using the dynamic threshold, and robust linear regression analysis results (RANSAC, see Methods). Gray shaded areas indicate confidence intervals of the regression fits. n = 5 fish in a,d,e,g,h, same fish as in Fig. 4. n = 8 model runs in b,c,e. All error bars are mean ± s.e.m. over simulated trials in b, model runs in c, or fish in g. P values in c,g are based on one-sided t-tests comparing differences to zero. Asterisks (*) in c,g indicate significance (*P < *0.05, or *P < 0.001).

Extended Data Fig. 7 Speculative network model implementation of urgency-related signals in freely swimming larval zebrafish.

a, Network model as in Fig. 3k but with inhibitory bout clock attached to the dynamic threshold clusters. We speculate that the bout clock or the system for keeping balance act as urgency-related signals here. These signals lead to rapidly collapsing bounds, allowing for spontaneous swimming. Also, in this model, each simulated bout induced opposing visual feedback, as in Fig. 1g–l. b, Copy of behavior data from Fig. 1b–f. c, Network model simulation results, quantified as in Fig. 1b–f, h–l. n = 8 model runs in c. All error bars are mean ± s.e.m. over fish in b or model runs in c. Asterisks in b indicate significance (*P < 0.05, *P < 0.01, or *P < 0.001). See Fig. 1b–f for more details on P values and statistics.

Supplementary information

Reporting Summary

Supplementary Video 1

Freely swimming larval zebrafish with example dot motion stimulus. We present 5 s of 0% coherence, followed by 10 s of 25% coherence and 5 s of 0% coherence. Dot motion is always perpendicular (rightward or leftward) locked to the body orientation of the fish. Dots have a short lifetime and stochastically disappear and reappear at a random location within the arena. Left panel shows the perspective of the experimenter (world-centric view). Right panel shows the visual stimulus from the perspective of the fish (fish-centric view). Note that sizes are not to scale. The body length of a larva zebrafish at this age (6 days post fertilization) is about 4 mm. The diameter of the visual arena is 12 cm.

Supplementary Video 2

Simulation of the leaky integrator model with visual feedback for freely swimming larval zebrafish. We present different levels of coherence (moving rightward or leftward), indicated by the orange and blue shaded areas. Bouts are initiated by a stochastic bout clock operating at a low probability below the threshold and a higher probability above it. Orange dots indicate bouts to the right, blue dots indicate bouts to the left. Note that in this model, each bout slightly resets the integrator, mimicking self-created optic flow and visual noise.

Supplementary Video 3

Simulation of the stochastic model for freely swimming larval zebrafish. Same as in Supplementary Video 2 but for the stochastic model. Note that threshold crossing events are rarely visible because we slightly low-pass filter the sensory variable over time. Otherwise, the trace would not be resolvable in the movie.

Supplementary Video 4

Simulation of the stochastic model with motor memory for freely swimming larval zebrafish. Same as in Supplementary Video 2 but for the stochastic model with motor memory. Bouts are initiated according to the same rules as in the stochastic model. Additionally, a motor memory component allows larvae to sometimes simply repeat the last motor action.

Supplementary Video 5

Simulation of the leaky integrator model without visual feedback for freely swimming larval zebrafish. Same as in Supplementary Video 2 but for a simpler version of the bounded leaky integrator model in which we did not include self-created optic flow or visual noise.

Supplementary Video 6

Simulation of the non-leaky integrator model with reset and motor memory for freely swimming larval zebrafish. Same as in Supplementary Video 2 but for an integrator model without a leak component in which bouts completely reset the sensory variable to zero. Similarly, as in Supplementary Video 4, an additional motor memory component allows larvae to sometimes repeat the last motor action.

Supplementary Video 7

Simulation of the leaky integrator model for head-fixed larval zebrafish. Following the behavior of head-fixed larvae, bouts (orange and blue dots) are only triggered when the integrator variable reaches the threshold and not stochastically. We present different levels of coherence (moving rightward or leftward), indicated by the orange and blue shaded areas. Note that whenever we detect a bout, we immediately set coherence levels to zero, as in the experiments.

Supplementary Video 8

Simulation of the stochastic model for head-fixed larval zebrafish. Same as in Supplementary Video 7 but for the stochastic model. Note that threshold crossing events are rarely visible because we slightly low-pass filter the sensory variable over time. Otherwise, the trace would not be resolvable in the movie.

Supplementary Video 9

Simulation of the non-leaky integrator model with reset and motor memory for head-fixed larval zebrafish. Same as in Supplementary Video 7 but for an integrator model without a leak component in which bouts completely reset the sensory variable to zero.

Supplementary Video 10

Denoised activity in the anterior hindbrain during motion integration. We created the video by reconstituting fluorescent activity from the segmented and calcium-extracted neuronal activity using the CaImAn framework. The video is not aligned to visual stimulation onset or bout events and is slightly sped up to enhance the visibility of bilateral circuit dynamics.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bahl, A., Engert, F. Neural circuits for evidence accumulation and decision making in larval zebrafish. Nat Neurosci 23, 94–102 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0534-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0534-9

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing