Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Perspective
  • Published:

Understanding the development of reward learning through the lens of meta-learning

Abstract

Determining how environments shape how people learn is central to understanding individual differences in goal-directed behaviour. Studies of the effects of early-life adversity on reward learning have revealed that the environments that infants and children experience exert lasting influences on reward-guided behaviour. However, the varied findings from this research are difficult to reconcile under a unified computational account. Studies of adaptive reinforcement learning have demonstrated that learning algorithms and parameters dynamically adapt to support reward-guided behaviour in varied contexts, but this body of research has largely focused on learning that proceeds within the short timeframes of experimental tasks. In this Perspective, we argue that, to understand how the structure of experienced environments shapes reward learning across development, computational accounts of the effects of environmental statistics on reinforcement learning need to be extended to encompass learning across multiple nested timescales of experience. To this end, we consider the development of reward learning through the lens of meta-learning models, in particular meta-reinforcement learning. This computational formalization can inspire new hypotheses and methods for empirical research to understand how features of experienced environments give rise to individual differences in learning and adaptive behaviour across development.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Learning to reinforcement learn over multiple timescales.
Fig. 2: Development through the lens of meta-reinforcement learning.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Scott, L. S., Pascalis, O. & Nelson, C. A. A domain-general theory of the development of perceptual discrimination. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 197–201 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Scott, L. S. & Monesson, A. The origin of biases in face perception. Psychol. Sci. 20, 676–680 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Werker, J. F. & Tees, R. C. Cross-language speech perception: evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant. Behav. Dev. 7, 49–63 (1984).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Hospodar, C. M., Hoch, J. E., Lee, D. K., Shrout, P. E. & Adolph, K. E. Practice and proficiency: factors that facilitate infant walking skill. Dev. Psychobiol. 63, e22187 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Saccani, R., Valentini, N. C., Pereira, K. R., Müller, A. B. & Gabbard, C. Associations of biological factors and affordances in the home with infant motor development. Pediatr. Int. 55, 197–203 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Sheridan, M. A., Peverill, M., Finn, A. S. & McLaughlin, K. A. Dimensions of childhood adversity have distinct associations with neural systems underlying executive functioning. Dev. Psychopathol. 29, 1777–1794 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Amso, D., Salhi, C. & Badre, D. The relationship between cognitive enrichment and cognitive control: a systematic investigation of environmental influences on development through socioeconomic status. Dev. Psychobiol. 61, 159–178 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Harlow, H. F. The formation of learning sets. Psychol. Rev. 56, 51–65 (1949).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Nussenbaum, K., Velez, J. A., Washington, B. T., Hamling, H. E. & Hartley, C. A. Flexibility in valenced reinforcement learning computations across development. Child Dev. 93, 1601–1615 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Behrens, T. E. J., Woolrich, M. W., Walton, M. E. & Rushworth, M. F. S. Learning the value of information in an uncertain world. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 1214–1221 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Gagne, C., Zika, O., Dayan, P. & Bishop, S. J. Impaired adaptation of learning to contingency volatility in internalizing psychopathology. eLife 9, e61387 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Browning, M., Behrens, T. E., Jocham, G., O’Reilly, J. X. & Bishop, S. J. Anxious individuals have difficulty learning the causal statistics of aversive environments. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 590–596 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Hanson, J. L., Williams, A. V., Bangasser, D. A. & Peña, C. J. Impact of early life stress on reward circuit function and regulation. Front. Psychiatry 12, 744690 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Galván, A. Neural plasticity of development and learning. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 879–890 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Wilkinson, M. P., Slaney, C. L., Mellor, J. R. & Robinson, E. S. J. Investigation of reward learning and feedback sensitivity in non-clinical participants with a history of early life stress. PLoS One 16, e0260444 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Birn, R. M., Roeber, B. J. & Pollak, S. D. Early childhood stress exposure, reward pathways, and adult decision making. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 13549–13554 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Dorfman, H. M. & Gershman, S. J. Controllability governs the balance between Pavlovian and instrumental action selection. Nat. Commun. 10, 5826 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Botvinick, M. et al. Reinforcement learning, fast and slow. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 408–422 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Li, Z., Zhou, F., Chen, F. & Li, H. Meta-SGD: learning to learn quickly for few-shot learning. Preprint at arXiv https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1707.09835 (2017).

  20. Wang, J. X. et al. Prefrontal cortex as a meta-reinforcement learning system. Nat. Neurosci. 21, 860–868 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Wang, J. X. et al. Learning to reinforcement learn. Preprint at arXiv https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1611.05763 (2016).

  22. Duan, Y. et al. RL2: fast reinforcement learning via slow reinforcement learning. Preprint at arXiv https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1611.02779 (2016).

  23. Weng, L. Meta Reinforcement Learning https://lilianweng.github.io/posts/2019-06-23-meta-rl/ (2019).

  24. Langdon, A. et al. Meta-learning, social cognition and consciousness in brains and machines. Neural Netw. 145, 80–89 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Binz, M. et al. Meta-learned models of cognition. Behav. Brain Sci. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X23003266 (2023).

  26. Schaul, T. & Schmidhuber, J. Metalearning. Scholarpedia J. 5, 4650 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Wang, J. X. Meta-learning in natural and artificial intelligence. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 38, 90–95 (2021).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Lansdell, B. J. & Kording, K. P. Towards learning-to-learn. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 29, 45–50 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Finn, C., Abbeel, P. & Levine, S. Model-agnostic meta-learning for fast adaptation of deep networks. In Proc. 34th International Conference on Machine Learning (eds Precup, D. & Teh, Y. W.) 70, 1126–1135 (PMLR, 2017).

  30. Doya, K. Metalearning and neuromodulation. Neural Netw. 15, 495–506 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Griffiths, T. L. et al. Doing more with less: meta-reasoning and meta-learning in humans and machines. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 29, 24–30 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Behrens, T. E. J. et al. What is a cognitive map? Organizing knowledge for flexible behavior. Neuron 100, 490–509 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Crowley, K. & Siegler, R. S. Explanation and generalization in young children’s strategy learning. Child Dev. 70, 304–316 (1999).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Bielaczyc, K., Pirolli, P. L. & Brown, A. L. Training in self-explanation and self-regulation strategies: investigating the effects of knowledge acquisition activities on problem solving. Cogn. Instr. 13, 221–252 (1995).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Bakst, L. & McGuire, J. T. Experience-driven recalibration of learning from surprising events. Cognition 232, 105343 (2023).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Dubey, R., Grant, E., Luo, M., Narasimhan, K. & Griffiths, T. Connecting context-specific adaptation in humans to meta-learning. Preprint at https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2011.13782 (2020).

  37. Verbeke, P. & Verguts, T. Humans adaptively select different computational strategies in different learning environments. Preprint at bioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.01.27.525944 (2023).

  38. Werchan, D. M., Collins, A. G. E., Frank, M. J. & Amso, D. 8-month-old infants spontaneously learn and generalize hierarchical rules. Psychol. Sci. 26, 805–815 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Mark, S., Moran, R., Parr, T., Kennerley, S. W. & Behrens, T. E. J. Transferring structural knowledge across cognitive maps in humans and models. Nat. Commun. 11, 4783 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Brown, A., Kane, M. J. & Echols, C. H. Young children’s mental models determine analogical transfer across problems with a common goal structure. Cogn. Dev. 1, 103–121 (1986).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Nussenbaum, K. et al. Causal information‐seeking strategies change across childhood and adolescence. Cognit. Sci. 44, e12888 (2020).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Kuhn, D. & Phelps, E. The development of problem-solving strategies. Adv. Child Dev. Behav. 17, 1–44 (1982).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Rescorla, R. A. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement. Classical Conditioning Curr. Res. Theory 2, 64–69 (1972).

    Google Scholar 

  44. Sutton, R. S. & Barto, A. G. Reinforcement Learning. An Introduction (MIT Press, 1998).

  45. Kool, W., Gershman, S. J. & Cushman, F. A. Cost-benefit arbitration between multiple reinforcement-learning systems. Psychol. Sci. 28, 1321–1333 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Ruel, A., Devine, S. & Eppinger, B. Resource-rational approach to meta-control problems across the lifespan. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 12, e1556 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Raab, H. A., Goldway, N., Foord, C. & Hartley, C. A. Adolescents flexibly adapt action selection based on controllability inferences. Learn. Mem. 31, a053901 (2024).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Salter Ainsworth, M. D. The Bowlby-Ainsworth attachment theory. Behav. Brain Sci. 1, 436–438 (1978).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Diederen, K. M. J. & Schultz, W. Scaling prediction errors to reward variability benefits error-driven learning in humans. J. Neurophysiol. 114, 1628–1640 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Payzan-LeNestour, E. & Bossaerts, P. Risk, unexpected uncertainty, and estimation uncertainty: Bayesian learning in unstable settings. PLoS Comput. Biol. 7, e1001048 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Piray, P. & Daw, N. D. A model for learning based on the joint estimation of stochasticity and volatility. Nat. Commun. 12, 6587 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Dayan, P., Kakade, S. & Montague, P. R. Learning and selective attention. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1218–1223 (2000).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Kalman, R. E. A new approach to linear filtering and prediction problems. J. Basic Eng. 82, 35–45 (1960).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Soltani, A. & Izquierdo, A. Adaptive learning under expected and unexpected uncertainty. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 20, 635–644 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Nassar, M. R., Wilson, R. C., Heasly, B. & Gold, J. I. An approximately Bayesian delta-rule model explains the dynamics of belief updating in a changing environment. J. Neurosci. 30, 12366–12378 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. McGuire, J. T., Nassar, M. R., Gold, J. I. & Kable, J. W. Functionally dissociable influences on learning rate in a dynamic environment. Neuron 84, 870–881 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Costa, V. D., Tran, V. L., Turchi, J. & Averbeck, B. B. Reversal learning and dopamine: a Bayesian perspective. J. Neurosci. 35, 2407–2416 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Mathys, C., Daunizeau, J., Friston, K. J. & Stephan, K. E. A Bayesian foundation for individual learning under uncertainty. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5, 39 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Piray, P. & Daw, N. D. A simple model for learning in volatile environments. PLoS Comput. Biol. 16, e1007963 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Farashahi, S. et al. Metaplasticity as a neural substrate for adaptive learning and choice under uncertainty. Neuron 94, 401–414.e6 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Nassar, M. R. et al. Rational regulation of learning dynamics by pupil-linked arousal systems. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 1040–1046 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Cazé, R. D. & van der Meer, M. A. A. Adaptive properties of differential learning rates for positive and negative outcomes. Biol. Cybern. 107, 711–719 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Louie, K. & Glimcher, P. W. Efficient coding and the neural representation of value. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1251, 13–32 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Dabney, W. et al. A distributional code for value in dopamine-based reinforcement learning. Nature 577, 671–675 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Gershman, S. J. Do learning rates adapt to the distribution of rewards? Psychonomic Bull. Rev. 22, 1320–1327 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Daw, N. D., Kakade, S. & Dayan, P. Opponent interactions between serotonin and dopamine. Neural Netw. 15, 603–616 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Frank, M. J., Seeberger, L. C. & O’Reilly, R. C. By carrot or by stick: cognitive reinforcement learning in parkinsonism. Science 306, 1940–1943 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Lefebvre, G., Lebreton, M., Meyniel, F., Bourgeois-Gironde, S. & Palminteri, S. Behavioural and neural characterization of optimistic reinforcement learning. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 0067 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Niv, Y., Edlund, J. A., Dayan, P. & O’Doherty, J. P. Neural prediction errors reveal a risk-sensitive reinforcement-learning process in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 32, 551–562 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Rosenbaum, G., Grassie, H. & Hartley, C. A. Valence biases in reinforcement learning shift across adolescence and modulate subsequent memory. eLife 11, e64620 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Chambon, V. et al. Information about action outcomes differentially affects learning from self-determined versus imposed choices. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 1067–1079 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Palminteri, S., Lefebvre, G., Kilford, E. J. & Blakemore, S.-J. Confirmation bias in human reinforcement learning: evidence from counterfactual feedback processing. PLoS Comput. Biol. 13, e1005684 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Habicht, J., Bowler, A., Moses-Payne, M. E. & Hauser, T. U. Children are full of optimism, but those rose-tinted glasses are fading — reduced learning from negative outcomes drives hyperoptimism in children. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 151, 1843–1853 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Villano, W. J. et al. Individual differences in naturalistic learning link negative emotionality to the development of anxiety. Sci. Adv. 9, eadd2976 (2023).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Cools, R. et al. Striatal dopamine predicts outcome-specific reversal learning and its sensitivity to dopaminergic drug administration. J. Neurosci. 29, 1538–1543 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Michely, J., Eldar, E., Erdman, A., Martin, I. M. & Dolan, R. J. Serotonin modulates asymmetric learning from reward and punishment in healthy human volunteers. Commun. Biol. 5, 812 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Cools, R., Robinson, O. J. & Sahakian, B. Acute tryptophan depletion in healthy volunteers enhances punishment prediction but does not affect reward prediction. Neuropsychopharmacology 33, 2291–2299 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Tanaka, S. C. et al. Serotonin affects association of aversive outcomes to past actions. J. Neurosci. 29, 15669–15674 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. den Ouden, H. E. M. et al. Dissociable effects of dopamine and serotonin on reversal learning. Neuron 80, 1090–1100 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Moscarello, J. M. & Hartley, C. A. Agency and the calibration of motivated behavior. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 725–735 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Ligneul, R. Prediction or causation? Towards a redefinition of task controllability. Trends Cogn. Sci. 25, 431–433 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Raab, H. A., Foord, C., Ligneul, R. & Hartley, C. A. Developmental shifts in computations used to detect environmental controllability. PLoS Comput. Biol. 18, e1010120 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Ligneul, R., Mainen, Z. F., Ly, V. & Cools, R. Stress-sensitive inference of task controllability. Nat. Hum. Behav. 6, 812–822 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Csifcsák, G., Melsæter, E. & Mittner, M. Intermittent absence of control during reinforcement learning interferes with Pavlovian bias in action selection. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 32, 646–663 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Dorfman, H. M., Bhui, R., Hughes, B. L. & Gershman, S. J. Causal inference about good and bad outcomes. Psychol. Sci. 30, 516–525 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  86. Cohen, A. O., Nussenbaum, K., Dorfman, H. M., Gershman, S. J. & Hartley, C. A. The rational use of causal inference to guide reinforcement learning strengthens with age. NPJ Sci. Learn. 5, 16 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  87. Pulcu, E. & Browning, M. Affective bias as a rational response to the statistics of rewards and punishments. eLife 6, e27879 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  88. Dorfman, H. M. et al. Causal inference gates corticostriatal learning. J. Neurosci. 41, 6892–6904 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  89. O’Doherty, J. et al. Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning. Science 304, 452–454 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Amat, J. et al. Medial prefrontal cortex determines how stressor controllability affects behavior and dorsal raphe nucleus. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 365–371 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Gershman, S. J., Guitart-Masip, M. & Cavanagh, J. F. Neural signatures of arbitration between Pavlovian and instrumental action selection. PLoS Comput. Biol. 17, e1008553 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  92. Palminteri, S. & Lebreton, M. The computational roots of positivity and confirmation biases in reinforcement learning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 26, 607–621 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Langer, E. J. The illusion of control. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 32, 311–328 (1975).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  94. Lefebvre, G., Summerfield, C. & Bogacz, R. A normative account of confirmation bias during reinforcement learning. Neural Comput. 34, 307–337 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  95. Huys, Q. J. M. & Dayan, P. A Bayesian formulation of behavioral control. Cognition 113, 314–328 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Schubert, J. A., Jagadish, A. K., Binz, M. & Schulz, E. A rational analysis of the optimism bias using meta-reinforcement learning. In 2023 Conference on Cognitive Computational Neuroscience 557–559 (2023).

  97. Greenough, W. T., Black, J. E. & Wallace, C. S. in Brain Development and Cognition: A Reader 2nd ed., 186–216 (Wiley, 2008).

  98. Knudsen, E. I. Sensitive periods in the development of the brain and behavior. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1412–1425 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Gabard-Durnam, L. & McLaughlin, K. A. Sensitive periods in human development: charting a course for the future. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 36, 120–128 (2020).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  100. Hensch, T. K. Critical period regulation. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 549–579 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Takesian, A. E. & Hensch, T. K. Balancing plasticity/stability across brain development. Prog. Brain Res. 207, 3–34 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Fawcett, T. W. & Frankenhuis, W. E. Adaptive explanations for sensitive windows in development. Front. Zool. 12, S3 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  103. Golarai, G. & Ghahremani, D. G. The development of race effects in face processing from childhood through adulthood: neural and behavioral evidence. Dev. Sci. 24, e13058 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. Kuhl, P. K. et al. Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: new data and native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e). Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 363, 979–1000 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Lin, W. C., Delevich, K. & Wilbrecht, L. A role for adaptive developmental plasticity in learning and decision making. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 36, 48–54 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  106. Anzures, G. et al. Developmental origins of the other-race effect. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22, 173–178 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  107. Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F.-M. & Liu, H.-M. Foreign-language experience in infancy: effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100, 9096–9101 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  108. Best, C. T., McRoberts, G. W., LaFleur, R. & Silver-Isenstadt, J. Divergent developmental patterns for infants’ perception of two nonnative consonant contrasts. Infant. Behav. Dev. 18, 339–350 (1995).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  109. Kelly, D. J. et al. The other-race effect develops during infancy: evidence of perceptual narrowing. Psychol. Sci. 18, 1084–1089 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A. & Lambert, H. K. Childhood adversity and neural development: deprivation and threat as distinct dimensions of early experience. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 47, 578–591 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  111. Ellis, B. J., Sheridan, M. A., Belsky, J. & McLaughlin, K. A. Why and how does early adversity influence development? Toward an integrated model of dimensions of environmental experience. Dev. Psychopathol. 34, 447–471 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  112. Mehta, M. A. et al. Hyporesponsive reward anticipation in the basal ganglia following severe institutional deprivation early in life. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 2316–2325 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Hanson, J. L. et al. Behavioral problems after early life stress: contributions of the hippocampus and amygdala. Biol. Psychiatry 77, 314–323 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  114. Dillon, D. G. et al. Childhood adversity is associated with left basal ganglia dysfunction during reward anticipation in adulthood. Biol. Psychiatry 66, 206–213 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  115. Park, A. T. et al. Early childhood stress is associated with blunted development of ventral tegmental area functional connectivity. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 47, 100909 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  116. Marusak, H. A., Hatfield, J. R. B., Thomason, M. E. & Rabinak, C. A. Reduced ventral tegmental area–hippocampal connectivity in children and adolescents exposed to early threat. Biol. Psychiatry Cognit. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 2, 130–137 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  117. Fareri, D. S. et al. Altered ventral striatal-medial prefrontal cortex resting-state connectivity mediates adolescent social problems after early institutional care. Dev. Psychopathol. 29, 1865–1876 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  118. Evans, G. W., Li, D. & Whipple, S. S. Cumulative risk and child development. Psychol. Bull. 139, 1342–1396 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  119. Ellis, B. J., Bianchi, J., Griskevicius, V. & Frankenhuis, W. E. Beyond risk and protective factors: an adaptation-based approach to resilience. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 561–587 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  120. Frankenhuis, W. E., Panchanathan, K. & Nettle, D. Cognition in harsh and unpredictable environments. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 7, 76–80 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  121. Ellwood-Lowe, M. E., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S. & Bunge, S. A. Brain network coupling associated with cognitive performance varies as a function of a child’s environment in the ABCD study. Nat. Commun. 12, 7183 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  122. Amso, D. Neighborhood poverty and brain development: adaptation or maturation, fixed or reversible? JAMA Netw. Open 3, e2024139 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  123. Burk, D. C. & Averbeck, B. B. Environmental uncertainty and the advantage of impulsive choice strategies. PLoS Comput. Biol. 19, e1010873 (2023).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  124. Frankenhuis, W. E. & Gopnik, A. Early adversity and the development of explore-exploit tradeoffs. Trends Cogn. Sci. 27, 616–630 (2023).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  125. Santarelli, S. et al. Evidence supporting the match/mismatch hypothesis of psychiatric disorders. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 24, 907–918 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  126. Schmidt, M. V. Animal models for depression and the mismatch hypothesis of disease. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 330–338 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  127. Humphreys, K. L. et al. Exploration-exploitation strategy is dependent on early experience. Dev. Psychobiol. 57, 313–321 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  128. Harms, M. B., Shannon Bowen, K. E., Hanson, J. L. & Pollak, S. D. Instrumental learning and cognitive flexibility processes are impaired in children exposed to early life stress. Dev. Sci. 21, e12596 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  129. Hanson, J. L. et al. Early adversity and learning: implications for typical and atypical behavioral development. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 58, 770–778 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  130. Lloyd, A., McKay, R., Sebastian, C. L. & Balsters, J. H. Are adolescents more optimal decision-makers in novel environments? Examining the benefits of heightened exploration in a patch foraging paradigm. Dev. Sci. 24, e13075 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  131. Kamkar, N. H., Lewis, D. J., van den Bos, W. & Morton, J. B. Ventral striatal activity links adversity and reward processing in children. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 20–27 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  132. Smith, K. E. & Pollak, S. D. Early life stress and perceived social isolation influence how children use value information to guide behavior. Child Dev. 93, 804–814 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  133. Gerin, M. I. et al. A neurocomputational investigation of reinforcement-based decision making as a candidate latent vulnerability mechanism in maltreated children. Dev. Psychopathol. 29, 1689–1705 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  134. Zador, A. M. A critique of pure learning and what artificial neural networks can learn from animal brains. Nat. Commun. 10, 3770 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  135. Mnih, V. et al. Human-level control through deep reinforcement learning. Nature 518, 529–533 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  136. Harhen, N. C. & Bornstein, A. M. Interval timing as a computational pathway from early life adversity to affective disorders. Top. Cogn. Sci. 16, 92–112 (2024).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  137. Saxe, A. M., McClelland, J. L. & Ganguli, S. A mathematical theory of semantic development in deep neural networks. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 116, 11537–11546 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  138. Rumelhart, D. E., Hinton, G. E. & Williams, R. J. Learning representations by back-propagating errors. Nature 323, 533–536 (1986).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  139. Andrychowicz, M. et al. Learning to learn by gradient descent by gradient descent. Adv. Neural Inf. Process. Syst. 29, 3988–3996 (2016).

    Google Scholar 

  140. Bechtle, S. et al. Meta-learning via learned loss. In Proc. IEEE International Conference on Pattern Recognition https://doi.org/10.1109/ICPR48806.2021.9412010 (ICPR, 2021).

  141. Sutton, R. S. Adapting bias by gradient descent: an incremental version of delta-bar-delta. AAAI 92, 171–176 (1992).

    Google Scholar 

  142. Nichol, A., Achiam, J. & Schulman, J. On first-order meta-learning algorithms. Preprint at https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1803.02999 (2018).

  143. Hochreiter, S. & Schmidhuber, J. Long short-term memory. Neural Comput. 9, 1735–1780 (1997).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  144. Xu, Z. et al. Meta-gradient reinforcement learning with an objective discovered online. Adv. Neural Inf. Proc. Syst. 33, 15254–15264 (2020).

    Google Scholar 

  145. Ritter, S., Wang, J. X., Kurth-Nelson, Z. & Botvinick, M. Episodic control as meta-reinforcement learning. Preprint at bioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/360537 (2018).

  146. Hattori, R. et al. Meta-reinforcement learning via orbitofrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 26, 2182–2191 (2023).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  147. You, K., Long, M., Wang, J. & Jordan, M. I. How does learning rate decay help modern neural networks? Preprint at https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1908.01878 (2019).

  148. Frankenhuis, W. E. & Walasek, N. Modeling the evolution of sensitive periods. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 41, 100715 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  149. Xu, Z., van Hasselt, H. & Silver, D. Meta-gradient reinforcement learning. Preprint at https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1805.09801 (2018).

  150. Zahavy, T. et al. A self-tuning actor-critic algorithm. Adv. Neural Inf. Process. Syst. 33, 20913–20924 (2020).

    Google Scholar 

  151. Zheng, Z., Oh, J. & Satinder, S. On learning intrinsic rewards for policy gradient methods. Preprint at https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1804.06459 (2018).

  152. Sanders, B. & Becker-Lausen, E. The measurement of psychological maltreatment: early data on the Child Abuse and Trauma Scale. Child Abuse Negl. 19, 315–323 (1995).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  153. Rudolph, K. D. et al. Toward an interpersonal life-stress model of depression: the developmental context of stress generation. Dev. Psychopathol. 12, 215–234 (2000).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  154. Young, E. S., Frankenhuis, W. E. & Ellis, B. J. Theory and measurement of environmental unpredictability. Evol. Hum. Behav. 41, 550–556 (2020).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  155. Roy, D. et al. in Symbol Grounding and Beyond (eds. Vogt, P., Sugita, Y., Tuci, E. & Nehaniv, C.) 192–196 (Springer, 2006).

  156. Sullivan, J., Mei, M., Perfors, A., Wojcik, E. & Frank, M. C. SAYCam: a large, longitudinal audiovisual dataset recorded from the infant’s perspective. Open Mind 5, 20–29 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  157. Ugarte, E. & Hastings, P. Assessing unpredictability in caregiver-child relationships: insights from theoretical and empirical perspectives. Dev. Psychopathol. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000305 (2022).

  158. Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Kuchirko, Y. & Song, L. Why is infant language learning facilitated by parental responsiveness? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 23, 121–126 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  159. Ainsworth, M. D. S., Bell, S. M. & Stayton, D. F. in The Integration of a Child into a Social World (ed. Richards, M. P. M.) 316, 99–135 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1974).

  160. Csikszentmihalyi, M., Larson, R. & Prescott, S. The ecology of adolescent activity and experience. J. Youth Adolesc. 6, 281–294 (1977).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  161. Russell, M. A. & Gajos, J. M. Annual research review: ecological momentary assessment studies in child psychology and psychiatry. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 61, 376–394 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  162. Heller, A. S. et al. Association between real-world experiential diversity and positive affect relates to hippocampal–striatal functional connectivity. Nat. Neurosci. 23, 800–804 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  163. Saragosa-Harris, N. M. et al. Real-world exploration increases across adolescence and relates to affect, risk taking, and social connectivity. Psychol. Sci. 33, 1664–1679 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  164. Bath, K., Manzano-Nieves, G. & Goodwill, H. Early life stress accelerates behavioral and neural maturation of the hippocampus in male mice. Horm. Behav. 82, 64–71 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  165. Rice, C. J., Sandman, C. A., Lenjavi, M. R. & Baram, T. Z. A novel mouse model for acute and long-lasting consequences of early life stress. Endocrinology 149, 4892–4900 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  166. Ivy, A. S., Brunson, K. L., Sandman, C. & Baram, T. Z. Dysfunctional nurturing behavior in rat dams with limited access to nesting material: a clinically relevant model for early-life stress. Neuroscience 154, 1132–1142 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  167. Goodkin, F. Rats learn the relationship between responding and environmental events: an expansion of the learned helplessness hypothesis. Learn. Motiv. 7, 382–393 (1976).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  168. Overmier, J. B., Patterson, J. & Wielkiewicz, R. M. in Coping and Health (eds Levine, S. & Ursin, H.) 1–38 (Springer, 1980).

  169. Powell, S. B., Newman, H. A., McDonald, T. A., Bugenhagen, P. & Lewis, M. H. Development of spontaneous stereotyped behavior in deer mice: effects of early and late exposure to a more complex environment. Dev. Psychobiol. 37, 100–108 (2000).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  170. Marques, J. M. & Olsson, I. A. S. The effect of preweaning and postweaning housing on the behaviour of the laboratory mouse (Mus musculus). Lab. Anim. 41, 92–102 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  171. Ivy, A. S. et al. Hippocampal dysfunction and cognitive impairments provoked by chronic early-life stress involve excessive activation of CRH receptors. J. Neurosci. 30, 13005–13015 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  172. Moriceau, S., Shionoya, K., Jakubs, K. & Sullivan, R. M. Early-life stress disrupts attachment learning: the role of amygdala corticosterone, locus ceruleus corticotropin releasing hormone, and olfactory bulb norepinephrine. J. Neurosci. 29, 15745–15755 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  173. Hartley, C. A., Nussenbaum, K. & Cohen, A. O. Interactive development of adaptive learning and memory. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 3, 59–85 (2021).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  174. Zhihong Zeng, A. Survey of affect recognition methods: audio, visual, and spontaneous expressions, 2009. IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell. 31, 39–58 (2021).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  175. Belo, J. P. R., Azevedo, H., Ramos, J. J. G. & Romero, R. A. F. Deep Q-network for social robotics using emotional social signals. Front. Robot. AI 9, 880547 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  176. Qureshi, A. H., Nakamura, Y., Yoshikawa, Y. & Ishiguro, H. Intrinsically motivated reinforcement learning for human–robot interaction in the real-world. Neural Netw. 107, 23–33 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  177. Kuhn, D. A developmental model of critical thinking. Educ. Res. 28, 16–46 (1999).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  178. Kuhn, D. Education for Thinking (Harvard Univ. Press, 2005).

  179. Joshi, S., Li, Y., Kalwani, R. M. & Gold, J. I. Relationships between pupil diameter and neuronal activity in the locus coeruleus, colliculi, and cingulate cortex. Neuron 89, 221–234 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  180. Murphy, P. R., O’Connell, R. G., O’Sullivan, M., Robertson, I. H. & Balsters, J. H. Pupil diameter covaries with BOLD activity in human locus coeruleus. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 4140–4154 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  181. Reimer, J. et al. Pupil fluctuations track rapid changes in adrenergic and cholinergic activity in cortex. Nat. Commun. 7, 13289 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  182. Bouret, S. & Sara, S. J. Network reset: a simplified overarching theory of locus coeruleus noradrenaline function. Trends Neurosci. 28, 574–582 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  183. Cook, J. L. et al. Catecholaminergic modulation of meta-learning. eLife 8, e51439 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  184. Newcombe, N. S. What is neoconstructivism? neoconstructivism. Child Dev. Perspect. 5, 157–160 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  185. Newcombe, N. S. Cognitive development: changing views of cognitive change. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 4, 479–491 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  186. Westermann, G. et al. Neuroconstructivism. Dev. Sci. 10, 75–83 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  187. Karmiloff-Smith, A. Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science (MIT Press, 1995).

  188. Johnson, M. H. Functional brain development in infants: elements of an interactive specialization framework. Child Dev. 71, 75–81 (2000).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  189. Westermann, G., Sirois, S., Shultz, T. R. & Mareschal, D. Modeling developmental cognitive neuroscience. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 227–232 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  190. Mareschal, D. & Shultz, T. R. Generative connectionist networks and constructivist cognitive development. Cogn. Dev. 11, 571–603 (1996).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  191. Astle, D. E., Johnson, M. H. & Akarca, D. Toward computational neuroconstructivism: a framework for developmental systems neuroscience. Trends Cogn. Sci. 27, 726–744 (2023).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  192. Elman, J. L. Learning and development in neural networks: the importance of starting small. Cognition 48, 71–99 (1993).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  193. Munakata, Y. & McClelland, J. L. Connectionist models of development. Dev. Sci. 6, 413–429 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  194. Fahlman, S. E. The recurrent cascade-correlation architecture. Adv. Neural Inf. Process. Syst. 3, 190–196 (1990).

    Google Scholar 

  195. Mata, R., Josef, A. K. & Hertwig, R. Propensity for risk taking across the life span and around the globe. Psychol. Sci. 27, 231–243 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  196. Falk, A. et al. Global evidence on economic preferences. Q. J. Econ. 133, 1645–1692 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  197. Kidd, C., Palmeri, H. & Aslin, R. N. Rational snacking: young children’s decision-making on the marshmallow task is moderated by beliefs about environmental reliability. Cognition 126, 109–114 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  198. Yanaoka, K. et al. Cultures crossing: the power of habit in delaying gratification. Psychol. Sci. 33, 1172–1181 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  199. Amir, D. et al. The developmental origins of risk and time preferences across diverse societies. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 149, 650–661 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  200. Amir, D. & Jordan, M. R. The behavioral constellation of deprivation may be best understood as risk management. Behav. Brain Sci. 40, e316 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  201. Abebe, T. Reconceptualising children’s agency as continuum and interdependence. Soc. Sci. 8, 81 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  202. Henrich, J., Heine, S. J. & Norenzayan, A. The weirdest people in the world? Behav. Brain Sci. 33, 61–83 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  203. Nielsen, M., Haun, D., Kärtner, J. & Legare, C. H. The persistent sampling bias in developmental psychology: a call to action. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 162, 31–38 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  204. Tenenbaum, J. B., Kemp, C., Griffiths, T. L. & Goodman, N. D. How to grow a mind: statistics, structure, and abstraction. Science 331, 1279–1285 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  205. Wellman, H. M. & Gelman, S. A. Cognitive development: foundational theories of core domains. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 43, 337–375 (1992).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  206. Lake, B. M., Ullman, T. D., Tenenbaum, J. B. & Gershman, S. J. Building machines that learn and think like people. Behav. Brain Sci. 40, e253 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  207. Nettle, D., Frankenhuis, W. E. & Rickard, I. J. The evolution of predictive adaptive responses in human life history. Proc. Biol. Sci. 280, 20131343 (2013).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  208. Gogtay, N. et al. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 101, 8174–8179 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  209. Averbeck, B. B. Pruning recurrent neural networks replicates adolescent changes in working memory and reinforcement learning. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 119, e2121331119 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  210. Ajemian, R., D’Ausilio, A., Moorman, H. & Bizzi, E. A theory for how sensorimotor skills are learned and retained in noisy and nonstationary neural circuits. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, E5078–E5087 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  211. Yamins, D. L. K. & DiCarlo, J. J. Using goal-driven deep learning models to understand sensory cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 356–365 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  212. Findling, C. & Wyart, V. Computation noise promotes cognitive resilience to adverse conditions during decision-making. Preprint at bioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.06.10.145300 (2020).

  213. Plappert, M. et al. Parameter space noise for exploration. Preprint at:arXiv https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1706.01905 (2017).

  214. Fortunato, M. et al. Noisy networks for exploration. In Proc. International Conference on Learning Representations (ICLR) (2018).

  215. McIntosh, A. R. et al. The development of a noisy brain. Arch. Ital. Biol. 148, 323–337 (2010).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  216. Smith, L. B., Jayaraman, S., Clerkin, E. & Yu, C. The developing infant creates a curriculum for statistical learning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 325–336 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  217. Kidd, C. & Hayden, B. Y. The psychology and neuroscience of curiosity. Neuron 88, 449–460 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  218. Gottlieb, J., Oudeyer, P.-Y., Lopes, M. & Baranes, A. Information-seeking, curiosity, and attention: computational and neural mechanisms. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 585–593 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  219. Bengio, Y., Louradour, J., Collobert, R. & Weston, J. Curriculum learning. In Proc. 26th Annual International Conference on Machine Learning 41–48 (Association for Computing Machinery, 2009).

  220. Oudeyer, P.-Y. & Kaplan, F. What is intrinsic motivation? A typology of computational approaches. Front. Neurorobot. 1, 6 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  221. Forestier, S., Mollard, Y. & Oudeyer, P.-Y. Intrinsically motivated goal exploration processes with automatic curriculum learning. J. Mach. Learn. Res. 23, 1–41 (2022).

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Bruno Averbeck, Rheza Budiono, Nathaniel Daw, Nora Harhen and Akshay Jagadish for helpful feedback on the manuscript. Preparation of this manuscript was supported by the C.V. Starr Fellowship (to K.N.).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors conceptualized the article content. K.N. wrote the article draft and all authors edited the manuscript before submission.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Kate Nussenbaum or Catherine A. Hartley.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Peer review

Peer review information

Nature Reviews Psychology thanks Dorsa Amir, who co-reviewed with Annya Dahmani; Jane Wang; and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Nussenbaum, K., Hartley, C.A. Understanding the development of reward learning through the lens of meta-learning. Nat Rev Psychol (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-024-00304-1

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-024-00304-1

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing